by Jennifer Sheehy-Skeffington and Jessica Rea
How poverty affects
people’s decision-making
processes
This report summarises the most recent evidence on the relationship between socioeconomic
status and the psychological, social and cultural processes that underpin decision-making.
February 2017
www.jrf.org.uk
How poverty affects people’s
decision-making processes
Jennifer Sheehy-Skeffington and Jessica Rea
As poverty continues to be a feature of the social and economic landscape
in the United Kingdom, attention is turning towards the potentially
damaging role played by individual decisions made in low-income contexts.
This report summarises the most recent evidence on the relationship
between socioeconomic status and the psychological, social and cultural
processes that underpin decision-making.
The report summarises:
the results of 15 systematic reviews of recent evidence on the relationship between socioeconomic
status and psychological, social and cultural processes underpinning decision-making;
insights on the impact of poverty on thinking, behaviour patterns, facing challenges and engaging
with the social world;
a discussion of what these relationships mean for decision-making by those living in or near poverty;
how decision-making in contexts of poverty serves important immediate functions, even if it has
negative consequences for long-term outcomes; and
implications for interventions designed to empower those living in poverty to make decisions that
enhance their long-term well-being.
i
Contents
Executive summary 1
1 Introduction 5
2 A framework for understanding poverty and decision-making 7
3 Methodology 11
4 The influence of poverty on key thinking processes 13
5 The influence of poverty on key behavioural patterns 18
6 The influence of poverty on ways of navigating life’s challenges 25
7 The influence of poverty on appraisals of the social world 31
8 Implications for policies and interventions designed to reduce poverty 38
9 Conclusion 44
Glossary 45
References 48
Appendix 63
Acknowledgements 74
About the authors 74
List of tables
1 Boolean search terms by reported mechanism 64
2 Exclusion criteria 66
3 Screening and filtering process 67
4 Relevance decisions 68
5 Additional mechanisms in the exploratory list 69
6 Coding frame 71
7 Illustrative timeframe of work undertaken 73
List of figures
1 Conceptual framework for understanding decision-making in the context of
poverty
7
2 Flow diagram of the search, screening and filtering process for the evidence
review
12
1
Executive summary
As poverty continues to be a persistent feature of the social and economic landscape in the United
Kingdom, attention is increasingly turning towards the potentially damaging role played by individual
decisions made in low-income contexts. Stereotypes propagated in public opinion, media representations,
and even political discourse, often depict those on the lowest incomes or in receipt of government
benefits as people apparently making little effort to improve their socioeconomic condition. Emerging
research from academic social science and behaviourally focused government bodies is beginning to
replace this set of assumptions regarding decision-making in poverty with robust evidence. This report
summarises the most recent research on this topic, with a focus on the influence of poverty and low
socioeconomic status (SES) on the psychological, social and cultural processes that underpin decision-
making in a range of settings.
An approach to understanding decision-making in
poverty
Being in poverty means living without enough resources – both money and education – to meet one’s
needs and to participate fully in society. It is also usually accompanied by unreliability in the availability of
food, shelter and employment, and instability in one’s environment, both of which are experienced as
stressful. Such a constrained decision-making context triggers changes in the functioning of key
psychological, social and cultural processes. This project studied such processes as intermediary
mechanisms through which low SES might influence decision-making in a range of domains. By focusing
on a core set of underlying mechanisms underpinning decision-making, it offers insight into why
observed behavioural patterns are taking place, and thus how to address them in the most effective and
widely applicable manner.
Decision-making can also be understood in terms of underlying patterns, in terms of whether they serve
immediate or more distant goals. Specifically, many of the suboptimal decisions and behaviours associated
with low-income groups are characterised by a preferential focus on the present (as opposed to the
future), on the actual (as opposed to the hypothetical), on those socially close (as opposed to those
socially distant), and on the ‘here’ (as opposed to places far away). Whereas some decisions might have
harmful distal consequences, such as the influence of unhealthy eating on health in older age, they often
perform important proximal functions, such as providing immediate comfort or a sense of social
connectedness. This report thus proposes recasting decision-making patterns associated with contexts of
poverty from being suboptimal or resulting from psychosocial deficits, to being rational or adaptive when
considered in terms of the proximal functions they serve. In doing so, it offers a more nuanced
understanding of the behavioural dimension of poverty than has previously been available, and charts a
path for more sustainable interventions than have yet been attempted.
Conducting the evidence review
The project team conducted systematic reviews of the influence of poverty and low SES on the key
psychological, social and cultural mechanisms that underpin decision-making. Starting with a large-scale
search of all major publication databases from the social sciences, the researchers narrowed results down
to a final set of 226 papers that fit a clear set of inclusion criteria. The papers included in the final review
were original, quantitative empirical studies of the link between SES and one of a limited set of key
psychosocial mechanisms, conducted in an OECD country, and published between 2010 and 2016.
These papers were supplemented by unpublished work and recent review papers, to arrive at a snapshot
of what is currently known about poverty, decision-making and the mechanisms that underpin their
interrelationship.
The influence of poverty on key thinking processes
The first set of mechanisms considered involve thinking. There is robust evidence that exposure to
poverty or low SES while young is associated with poorer functioning on tasks measuring basic cognitive
2
processes. In particular, the poorer one’s socioeconomic background, the worse one is likely to perform
on measures of selective attention and inhibitory control, both of which are important for focusing on a
goal and resisting distracting alternatives that might derail one from achieving it.
SES is also correlated with higher-level thinking processes such as reasoning and learning, as reflected in
income gaps in performance on intelligence tests and academic achievement more generally. All of these
relationships have been observed across age groups, in studies that measure SES and cognitive
performance at the same time, and in studies that look at the impact of early family social class
background and later-developing thinking skills and ability. It is difficult to rule out the role of heritable
differences in cognitive skills in explaining at least part of this relationship. However, some experimental
studies have demonstrated the causal impact of the subjective experience of low SES on cognitive
performance and subsequent decision-making, suggesting that the situation of poverty can affect the
thinking processes of all people, regardless of intelligence level.
The influence of poverty on key behavioural patterns
The second set of mechanisms included in this review concerned behavioural patterns, such as whether
one is likely to act in line with one’s goals, and what kinds of risks one will take in order to achieve them.
Growing up in a family low in SES is linked to lower academic aspirations and motivation to learn, a link
possibly driven by the poorer quality school environments, and lower parental expectations, to which
many low-income children are exposed. Family-level poverty also seems to be predictive of a lesser
likelihood of making study and career choices based on intrinsic enjoyment, as opposed to instrumental
goals such as job security and salary.
Multiple studies report a positive association between SES and self-regulation, the tendency to act in line
with future goals when they compete with more immediate rewards. The fact that those in poverty tend
to focus on current (over future) rewards can lead to decisions that are damaging in the long term. It also
appears to be heightened by the anxiety caused by thinking about social comparison and the stigma
associated with social class.
Though it is commonly assumed that people living in low-income contexts engage in greater risk-taking,
this was not supported by the evidence reviewed. Rather, it seems that being exposed to poverty while
growing up makes one more averse to taking risks. This appears to change, however, when one’s needs
become acute, and the only way to meet them is to take a risk. Finally, though the behaviour of one’s
peers has a large impact on one’s own decisions, there is inconsistent evidence regarding the link
between SES and risk-related behaviours of peers in the domains of health and schooling.
In sum, poverty seems to influence behavioural patterns by shifting one's focus towards meeting
immediate needs and threats. The stereotype of behavioural norms in low-income groups facilitating
greater risk-taking and impulsivity at the cost of ambition and motivation is not supported by the
research reviewed.
The influence of poverty on ways of navigating life’s
challenges
The third set of mechanisms considered involve the psychosocial resources one draws on to navigate
life’s challenges. The most basic of these is how one evaluates one’s sense of worth – a self-appraisal
that was found to be more negative among people lower in SES, across a range of life stages and age
groups. Those lower in income and education perceive themselves to sit lower in their surrounding social
hierarchy – a perception that often has negative consequences, depending on the cultural context, and
whether one can draw psychosocial support from spiritual or social domains.
People’s judgment of their competence at learning new skills and succeeding in tasks is also robustly
positively correlated with SES, as is their perception that their actions will make a difference to how their
lives turn out. This has important consequences for academic performance and health behaviours, as the
less one feels that one’s actions are under one’s control, and will have an impact, the less invested one is
in behaving in line with future goals. Though the role of perceived self-efficacy in understanding income
3
differences in health outcomes has been supported by previous research, the review of recent studies
unearthed inconsistent results regarding the link between poverty and specifically health-related efficacy.
Finally, there is a fairly consistent relationship between poverty and how one responds to stress: those
lower in SES exhibit less effective coping styles in response to difficult situations, and this has negative
consequences for physical health and quality of life.
In sum, though poverty has been attributed to a lack of ambition, aspiration or the correct mindset,
recent evidence implies that it is the experience of life on a low income that leads to downward
reappraisal of one's ability to influence one's life outcomes – a psychological shift that is arguably rational
and adaptive.
The influence of poverty on appraisals of the social
world
The fourth and final set of mechanisms reviewed can be loosely characterised as ways of appraising the
social world. Children from poorer socioeconomic backgrounds report a lower sense of belonging at
school and greater exposure to negative incidents such as bullying or sexual harassment. These findings
might explain the robust association between living in poverty and demonstrating more aggressive, and
less co-operative, behaviour at primary and secondary school. Parents living in worse economic
conditions have harsher parenting styles, a pattern also connected to greater levels of child aggression,
which endures later in life. Growing up in poverty is also linked to lower general trust of others, perhaps
because of a lower sense of inclusion in society at large.
People lower in SES put a lesser weight on personal aspirations and achievement, in favour of helping
others and conforming to community traditions. Some studies also find that they have more compassion
and generosity than those higher in SES.
Looking at the findings together, it is possible that greater dependence on others makes those living in
poverty behave more positively to those who are socially close (as long as they are given the mental relief
to do so), but more negatively to strangers and institutions.
Implications for poverty reduction strategies
Though policy recommendations and interventions were not directly assessed in this review, there are a
number of insights it has produced that can be brought forward to aid in the struggle to reduce and
eliminate poverty. At a general conceptual level, the authors argue for the framing of
decision-making in
poverty as the product of the interaction of individual resources and powerful socioeconomic and cultural
contexts.
This means recognising the cognitive constraints of life in poverty, as well as the existence of
cognitive skills
that are specialised towards context-specific goals.
Moving beyond the cognitive, to the domains of motivation and behaviour, the report recommends
understanding decision-making in poverty as a shift in psychological focus, towards the here, the now,
the actual, and those socially close. This entails considering the proximal functions served by decisions
and behaviours observed in low-income contexts, and thus the ways in which they may be not only
suboptimal, but also rational and adaptive
.
To the extent that they ensure proximal functions are
addressed, interventions designed with this framing in mind will be more sustainable than interventions
focused only on reducing distal costs. To the extent that they focus on underlying mechanisms, they will
also impact a wider range of decision domains than they would if focused on one kind of decision alone.
There is a specific set of areas for intervention indicated by the evidence in this review, which involve
action not only at the individual, but also at the neighbourhood, institutional and wider societal levels.
There are also important areas where more research is needed, if this more nuanced understanding of
poverty and decision-making is to have a robust evidence base, and thus be compelling in policy and
public circles.
In conclusion, this report argues that studying decision-making in poverty can be done in a way that
acknowledges the agency and purposiveness of those experiencing it. Specifically, a focus on the
4
evidence for the influence of poverty on core psychosocial mechanisms, and the proximally-adaptive
nature of many decisions that result, can open the way to attempts to reduce poverty that address its
behavioural dimension in contextually-sensitive and empowering ways.
5
1 Introduction
Despite rapid economic growth in recent decades, and national recovery from the Great Recession,
poverty continues to be a striking feature of British society (MacInnes
et al
., 2015), as it is of all rich
countries today. Its causes are multifaceted, and have been summarised in terms of structural
characteristics of the economy, taxation and public spending decisions by government, and barriers to
the accessibility of public services faced by an increasingly diverse and ageing population (Goulden, 2015;
Loopstra
et al
., 2015). Neglected until recently is an attempt to understand the agency and experiences
of people living in or near poverty, and the role of their decisions and behaviours in securing a better
future for them and their families.
The relative paucity of research from social science and policy scholars on the topic of decision-making in
contexts of poverty has not hindered the frequent appearance of the topic in public and political
discourse. Indeed, whenever public opinion is consulted regarding financial or housing support for those
on low or no incomes, beliefs about the behaviour and motivations of those in poverty take centre stage
(Bullock
et al
., 2003; Gilens, 1999). Whether surveyed in a US or UK context, some of the most
commonly cited reasons for the persistence of poverty among groups at the bottom of society are that
those receiving government assistance spend their money unwisely, do not try hard enough to look for
work, and do not communicate to their children the value of education and of productive participation in
society (Bullock
et al
., 2003; Clery
et al
., 2013). This bias towards behavioural explanations for the
persistence of poverty is mirrored in media images of those in poverty which, through their focus on case
studies or fictitious communities (cf. soap operas such as
Emmerdale
), orient the audience to see poverty
as an individual rather than a structural phenomenon (Bullock
et al
., 2001; MacDonald
et al
., 2014;
McKendrick
et al
., 2008).
Moving from the public to the political sphere, the importance of individual effort in escaping poverty is a
central plank in UK Government efforts to reform the social welfare system (Department for Work and
Pensions, 2015). This is part of a wider growth in interest among policy-makers in the behavioural
dimension of socioeconomic issues, as evidenced by the formalisation of a role for behavioural science at
the heart of the UK and US governments (in the form of the UK Behavioural Insights Team and the
White House Social and Behavioural Sciences Team, respectively), and even the international
development agenda (World Bank, 2014). A positive feature of these developments is the
acknowledgement that we need to replace assumptions regarding decisions and behaviours associated
with poverty with robust empirical evidence.
To aid in this task, emerging research in applied social science fields is beginning to document the role of
decision-making in understanding adverse outcomes associated with poverty in the industrialised world.
In the field of public health, an important role has been assigned to behavioural patterns concerning diet,
exercise, smoking cessation and medication adherence, in exacerbating socioeconomic disparities in
health outcomes (Adler and Rehkopf, 2008; Pampel
et al
., 2010; Stringhini
et al
., 2010). In education,
differences in motivation and ability to create a home learning environment, to pay attention at school, or
to spend time on homework, have been found to explain part of the persistently observed gap in
academic achievement between income groups (Chowdry
et al
., 2011). In economics, decisions to take
out high-interest loans, and to spend limited money on gambling or non-essential goods, are highlighted
as worsening the financial situation of the most vulnerable (Bertrand
et al
., 2006).
The set of behaviours that might be pivotal either in increasing one’s risk of poverty, or in helping one
move towards financial stability and societal inclusion, spans a range of life domains. Such behaviours are
the product of decisions not only concerning one’s health and finances, or access to medical and
education services, but also regarding how one spends one’s leisure time, organises one’s family life,
chooses what products to buy, approaches one’s job, and selects where to live. Underlying the apparent
diversity in decision-making settings, however, is a commonality in the psychosocial processes that drive
decisions made within them. This report assumes that only through understanding the shared underlying
mechanisms that explain decisions made in the context of poverty can we hope to design interventions
that have broad impact.
6
As well as being broad in their impact, behaviourally focused anti-poverty interventions also need to be
sustainable. This means they will need to work in the wider context of people’s daily lives. This report
argues that this can only be achieved with full awareness of the functions served by decisions and
behaviours that at first seem suboptimal or unwise. That is, it recommends understanding decision-
making patterns associated with poverty as the product of psychosocial mechanisms that respond in
purposive and rational ways to the experience of life at the bottom of the socioeconomic spectrum.
This report first presents the overarching framework with which the authors approach the study of
decision-making in contexts of poverty. It then focuses on the most important mechanisms –
psychological, social, and cultural – that underpin decisions and behaviours in a range of life settings. In
attempting to understand how these psychosocial processes function in contexts of poverty, it presents a
systematic review of the evidence for the influence of poverty or low socioeconomic status on each
mechanism in turn. This set of reviews also considers how psychological, social and cultural processes are
influenced by poverty at different life stages, and which social groups might be more affected than
others.
After presenting overviews of research on how and whether poverty influences each set of mechanisms,
the report discusses what the findings might mean for decision-making in key life domains. Finally, it
considers policy and public service implications of the findings of the evidence review. The hope is that a
conceptually nuanced understanding of decision-making in the context of poverty will provide insights
that are useful to ongoing efforts from government and statutory bodies to design policies with human
behaviour in mind. It might also empower non-governmental organisations to design, implement and
lobby for anti-poverty strategies that increase the ability of those in the UK’s poorest communities to
make decisions that enhance their long-term well-being.
7
2 A framework for understanding
poverty and decision-making
Figure 1 outlines the conceptual framework guiding our understanding of decision-making in the context
of poverty. This chapter discusses each section of the framework in turn, highlighting the research on
which it is based, and how it shapes the interpretation of the findings of the evidence review.
Figure 1: Conceptual framework for understanding decision-making in the context
of poverty
The influence of characteristics of life in poverty on
psychological, social and cultural processes
The box shown at the top of Figure 1 considers the most important potential characteristics of daily life
in poverty or on a very low income, in the United Kingdom. By definition, living in or near poverty means
struggling to have enough money to meet essential needs such as food, clothing, secure shelter,
transport and personal care. More than this, living on a low income in a rich country often entails doing
without desirable goods that are seen as critical to full participation in society, such as a mobile phone,
clothes for special occasions, and opportunities to take part in social activities and school outings
8
(Goulden and D’Arcy, 2014). The poorer one is, the less educated one is, on average, such that poverty
brings with it obstacles to participation and advancement in the workforce that can persist through
generations. Educational and asset-based barriers to participation in society, along with rising levels of
economic inequality, mean that poverty is not only an experience of material deprivation, but of relative
deprivation, and with it a sense of social exclusion (Gordon
et al
., 2000).
In addition to poor quality of life, precariousness in income often brings with it instability in food supply,
shelter and employment, and the stresses of living in an unpredictable environment more generally
(Kushel
et al
., 2006). The most deprived neighbourhoods are also likely to be the more dangerous ones,
whether from frequent instances of aggression and violence, or greater exposure to environmental
toxins damaging to one’s health (Sampson, 2012). Unsurprisingly given stress, obstacles and poor quality
of life, living in poverty is associated with far lower levels of reported subjective well-being than living in
any other socioeconomic position (Haushofer and Fehr, 2014). It is through consideration of the features
of daily life in poverty that we begin to see a picture of what has been called its ‘psychological situation’
(Sheehy-Skeffington and Haushofer, 2014): the parameters and cues that make some behavioural
responses more facilitated, and thus more likely, than others (see also Steele and Sherman, 1999). The
framework thus next turns to the set of psychological, social and cultural processes that might be
influenced by the experience of being in poverty or low in socioeconomic status (SES).
As outlined in the next chapter, the evidence review began by considering a comprehensive set of
processes that might play a role in decision-making, before homing in on the core set of mechanisms
deemed most important to understanding decision-making in poverty, summarised in Figure 1. The initial
set included basic cognitive processes such as executive functioning, reasoning, and heuristics and biases,
as well as the neurological mechanisms underpinning them. In addition to cognition, it also included the
other two ‘building blocks’ of the mind – emotion and motivation – by considering processes such as
affective arousal, attitudinal mindsets and sense of personal efficacy. Other psychological processes
considered include the regulation of competing goals and one’s behavioural tendencies to take risks, as
well as key personality dimensions such as extraversion and conscientiousness.
Moving from psychological to social processes, the project team first considered verbal and nonverbal
communication, while also looking at social skills such as emotional intelligence and persuasive power.
Social perceptions and behaviours were considered both at the interpersonal and intergroup levels,
including how one behaves towards and appraises the actions not only of other members of one’s family
or neighbourhood, but also those of people in society at large.
Finally, the review considered the set of cultural processes most relevant to decision-making. This
included patterns of behaviour or norms that are widely spread in one’s sociocultural environment, and
the extent to which one lives in a community in which the self is seen as independent versus
interdependent. The review considered social representations of societal phenomena such as education
and social class, as well as whether one’s cultural group values goals such as being well-educated, working
hard and relating to others.
As outlined later in this report, a key subset of these psychological, social and cultural processes are
influenced by life in poverty, with consequences for decision-making in a range of domains. By referring
to these processes as psychosocial mechanisms, we can investigate the extent to which they mediate the
influence of poverty on the kinds of decisions and behaviours that are observed more commonly in low-
income than high-income groups, and thus claimed to play a role in the persistence of poverty in the
industrialised world. As behaviour change specialists have pointed out (Madrian, 2014), only by
understanding underlying mechanisms of a behavioural pattern can we plan interventions that lead to
improved behaviour and life situations in a sustained manner. Furthermore, because the same
psychosocial mechanism (such as risk-taking) might play out in a similar way across multiple domains
(such as gambling, heavy drinking and anti-social behaviour), an intervention targeted towards the
mechanism can have useful ‘spillover’ effects from one to many other kinds of decisions (see Dolan and
Galizzi, 2015).
The functional landscape of decision-making in poverty
In order to arrive at the connection between an infinite number of potential decisions and a small set of
underlying psychosocial mechanisms, it is worth drawing out the general patterns that might shape
9
decision-making in a range of contexts. The current framework achieves this with the use of insights
from construal level theory, a model from social psychology that looks at decisions as a product of a
changeable psychological state described as one’s information-processing style (see Trope and Liberman,
2010). According to this approach, at any one time, the mind is oriented towards processing information
that is at varying degrees of ‘psychological distance’ from the self at that moment. Information that is
psychologically ‘distal’ is about the future, about the hypothetical, about socially distant people, or about
far-away places. Information that is psychologically ‘proximal’, on the other hand, concerns the present,
the actual, the ‘us’, and the ‘here’. Researchers in the tradition of construal level theory have amassed a
large body of evidence that as the mind shifts between focusing on stimuli or decisions that are distal
versus proximal, its style of information-processing changes with it. Decisions focused on distal contexts
are better served by more abstract, goal-focused thinking, whereas decisions focused on proximal
contexts, such as in the case of an immediate threat, are better served by more concrete,
implementation-focused thinking (Trope and Liberman, 2010). It is thus worth considering whether the
concept of psychological distance might be a useful way to organise the many kinds of decisions of
interest in the context of poverty.
A review of existing insights on decision-making patterns associated with poverty suggests that this
organising framework fits it well (see Sheehy-Skeffington, in press; Sheehy-Skeffington and Haushofer,
2014). Emerging theoretical perspectives on poverty and decision-making are already converging on the
observation that what characterises a range of behaviours in low-income contexts is a focus on the
present at the cost of the future – that is, on proximal goals on the temporal dimension of psychological
distance (Mullainathan and Shafir, 2013; Pepper and Nettle, under review; Sheehy-Skeffington, in press).
Seen through this lens, unhealthy eating is a product of privileging short-term pleasure over long-term
health, just as taking out high-interest loans favours meeting an immediate financial need as opposed to
future needs, and skipping school may offer enjoyment now at the cost of future academic performance.
A separate set of research from social psychology highlights what can be called the existential dimension
of psychological distance: it finds that being low in a social hierarchy leads to the neglect of hypothetical
goals for the sake of focusing on what is actual, or tangible (Keltner
et al
., 2003). This might translate into
those lower in socioeconomic standing being more focused on actual threats and barriers, rather than
possible rewards and dreams – a mindset that we know hampers academic performance among those
low in social class (Oyserman
et al
., 2011; see also Dweck and Leggett, 1988).
Moving from the self to others, there is consistent evidence from the field of political psychology that the
lower one is in SES, the more one is biased in one’s attitudes and behaviours towards members of one’s
own social group, as opposed to members of other groups (Wagner and Zick, 1995) – a pattern that fits
with a proximal focus on the social dimension of psychological distance. If being from a low social class
background is associated with more altruistic behaviour towards others (Piff
et al
., 2010), y
et al
so with
greater hostility towards strangers (Barefoot
et al
., 1991), this might be because poverty brings with it a
shift towards those who are socially close, at the expense of those who are socially distant. This is
supported by evidence that experiencing hunger leads to greater demands for resources for one’s social
group, but lesser likelihood to share resources with a stranger (Aarøe and Petersen, 2013).
There is less evidence that speaks directly to the link between poverty and the spatial dimension of
psychological distance. However, the finding that perceptions of low status can cause greater desire to
spend on status-displaying goods such as clothing and electronics (Rucker and Galinsky, 2008, 2009;
Sivanathan and Pettit, 2010) could be understood as a privileging of gains in local social standing over
gains in societal social standing. It has also been observed that those lower in SES are less likely to move
from their neighbourhood to a less deprived one when given the opportunity (South and Crowder, 1997),
suggestive of a focus on, and preference for, one’s local geographic proximity, as against exploring new
locations. Perhaps more importantly, the existence of spatial segregation by SES in the UK and other rich
countries (Musterd, 2005) implies that such a constriction in geographic focus may be externally imposed
through housing policy and discrimination, if not voluntarily chosen.
Overlaying the framework of construal level theory onto the set of findings on poverty and decision-
making enables us to do more than group them into a manageable number of categories. It also sheds
light on how what seem to be suboptimal decisions might not actually be irrational ones, and certainly
aren’t without a purpose. That is, there are very good reasons why one might need to shift into a
proximal mindset – focusing on the here, the now, the real, and those close – such as if one needs to
address a pressing threat or is in immediate need. Engaging a distal mindset, on the other hand, is
10
something of a privilege: it makes sense when one’s mind and time are freed up to contemplate future
goals and aspirations in a predictable world, but will not be of any help in navigating immediate challenges
in unstable environments (see also Frankenhuis and de Weerth, 2013; Frankenhuis
et al
., 2016).
Challenges, threats, instability, scarcity in time, money and mental space – all are characteristic of life on
a very low income, in stark contrast to the kind of middle-class contexts where a focus on distal goals is
instilled. Thus, one could argue that what appears self-destructive or unreasonable in a decision-making
pattern associated with poverty is, in fact, performing an important proximal function – one that is hard
to see from the perspective of social science observers writing from well beyond the world of poverty.
The boxes at the right of Figure 1 provide illustrations of the difference between the proximal versus
distal impact of decisions that one might be more likely to make if one grows up in a low-income context.
Though smoking cigarettes might harm one’s hypothetical health goals, it offers physical pleasure and
pain relief that is very tangible in the moment. Joining a neighbourhood gang is a decision that is linked
with consequences such as marginalisation from society at large (Thornberry, 2003), but provides a sense
of identity and bonding with socially close others. Though spending the bulk of one’s pay cheque on a
smartphone might ultimately lower one’s socioeconomic standing in society at large, it offers the
opportunity for greater local respect, in one’s neighbourhood or among one’s friendship group. Finally,
taking out a high-interest loan in order to pay for school uniforms might be financially damaging in the
long run, but it helps solves a pressing problem in the moment, thus relieving the stress of not being able
to provide for a child’s needs.
In using the above organising framework to form the backdrop of our understanding of decision-making
in poverty, the authors do not claim to try to fit all decisions and behaviours into a limited set of ‘types’.
Indeed, some decisions are just poor decisions, and are the product of the disrupted operation of
cognition rather than a functional focus on the proximal; this is represented by the dimension of ‘decision
quality’ in the box at the bottom left-hand corner of Figure 1. Nevertheless, it is our contention that
being alert to both the proximal and the distal impacts of decisions made in contexts of poverty enables a
much more nuanced understanding than does studying them as merely damaging or suboptimal. Indeed,
by considering the subjective purpose that a particular decision or behavioural pattern serves, one can
see how the only substitute behaviour that can have a more empowering, sustainable, long-term impact
is one that also ensures key proximal functions are addressed.
11
3 Methodology
The project team conducted a wide-ranging review of recent scientific evidence for the link between
poverty and psychological, social and cultural processes, as they are relevant for decision-making in key
life domains. The project consisted of a set of 15 evidence reviews, conducted in stages aligned with the
Cochrane and Campbell principles for the systematic review of interventions (see Davies and Boruch,
2001). A detailed account of the search and selection process, as well as a record of all final papers
included in the review, can be found in the Appendix.
The first phase consisted of consulting the most widely used academic databases (EBSCO, IBSS, Scopus,
Web of Science and Zetoc), which contain records from journals spanning the fields of psychology,
economics, sociology, anthropology, political science, education, medicine, and the social sciences more
generally. These databases were searched for journal articles, conference proceedings and dissertations
published from the beginning of 2010 to the beginning of 2016, which reported original empirical
studies, and which fit a combination of two sets of search terms. The first set of search terms referred to
poverty as construed broadly, including not only the words ‘poor’ and ‘poverty’, but also terms such as
‘low socioeconomic status’, ‘adversity’, and ‘marginalisation’. Thus, in order to capture linear effects of
relative deprivation and subjective social standing, and to consider those at risk of poverty as well as those
currently in poverty, the review considered the relationship between decision-making processes and low
socioeconomic status (SES) more generally. The poverty-related search terms were then combined with
a second set of search terms, depicting one of a large set of psychological, social or cultural mechanisms.
The initial list of 54 mechanisms was developed from a combination of psychological theory and
consideration of the existing literature, and was intended to cover all plausible intermediary processes
that might underlie decision-making and behaviour.
Once searches were conducted for each mechanism in turn, the large list of results was screened to
remove duplicates, and to ensure that only papers that matched the search terms as intended remained.
As the overarching research question concerns poverty within the context of the UK, studies conducted
in non-OECD countries (and in Mexico or Chile) were excluded. Studies conducted in all other OECD
countries were included due to the similarity of the economic national context, and the fact that the bulk
of research on this topic has been conducted in the US and Europe. Following the screening phase, the
team then engaged in a first phase of filtering, applying a set of exclusion criteria to the article titles, in
order to leave in only those papers that involved original quantitative assessment of the evidence for a
link between poverty and the mechanism in question.
At this stage, scores were given to each mechanism, depicting the number of articles pertaining to that
mechanism, the relevance of those articles to decision-making, and the theoretically derived importance
of that mechanism to decision-making in the context of poverty. Using this method, the project team
selected the 15 most important mechanisms to take forward to the second stage of the evidence review.
A second phase of filtering was then conducted based on article abstracts, resulting in the removal of
articles concerning only mechanisms outside of the selected set, articles that did not enable the
assessment of the relationship of SES with a mechanism (e.g. that included only participants from a low-
income context), and articles that did not meet the inclusion criteria of first stage filtering.
This procedure produced a short list of articles, each of which were then read fully and coded to extract
the key methodological details and findings (see Appendix for the coding frame). Though no articles were
excluded based on their methodological rigour, each article was assigned scores for ‘quality’ and
‘relevance’, informed by considerations of sample size and specific methodology (e.g. cross-sectional,
longitudinal, experimental), as well as applicability to the context of poverty in the UK. In terms of sample
size, the summaries below use the term ‘small’ to refer to sample sizes below 100, ‘medium’ to refer to
sample sizes between 100 and 1,000, ‘large’ to refer to sample sizes between 1,000 and 10,000, and
‘very large’ to refer to sample sizes larger than 10,000. In terms of study design, it is important to note
that a correlation between poverty and the functioning of a psychosocial mechanism cannot be taken to
infer a causal relationship from the former to the latter. At the same time, however, the random
assignment of socioeconomic conditions, as would be necessary to make causal claims, is neither ethically
nor practically feasible. The review thus included results of the limited set of studies that did attempt to
experimentally recreate some of the experiences associated with poverty (such as resource scarcity or
12
low subjective social status), summarising them separately to those studying real-life socioeconomic
conditions. This report restricts usage of causal language (such as ‘poverty leads to’ or ‘the effect of
poverty’) to such studies, intending other forms of description of relationships (such as ‘poverty is
associated with’ and ‘SES predicts’) not to imply any strict causal ordering.
In the process of article coding and evaluation, some final articles that did not meet the inclusion criteria
were identified and removed, resulting in the final list, consisting of 226 articles, of which 23 involved UK
samples. A summary of the search, screening and filtering process is provided in Figure 2.
Figure 2: Flow diagram of the search, screening and filtering process for the
evidence review
Although grey literature in the form of conference proceedings and dissertations did form around 15% of
the original search results, additional searches were conducted for reports published by organisations
including charities and academic and government organisations working on relevant topics. Networks in
relevant professional organisations (e.g. the American Psychological Association Committee on
Socioeconomic Status), as well as leading researchers in the area of poverty and decision-making, were
contacted via email to solicit unpublished findings. Social networking sites were also used to request
unpublished work from researchers in the field (e.g. the Poverty Network on ResearchGate).
Results of this additional search were combined with a set of theoretical and review papers on the topic
of poverty and decision-making more broadly, in order to provide insight into the links between the
mechanisms found to be affected by poverty and decisions in key life domains, and also to inform
recommendations for intervention.
13
4 The influence of poverty on key
thinking processes
Underpinning all decision-making are the building blocks of cognition: the psychological mechanisms
through which we process information from the world, and use it to orient our attitudes, judgments and
behaviours. Investigating the relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and cognitive processes is
thus a natural first place to start in the search for mechanisms underlying decision-making in poverty.
Cognition is more than what is commonly denoted by the word ‘thinking’. For before intelligence and
knowledge can be brought to bear in high-level processes such as reasoning, more basic processes need
to be doing their job. Specifically, the brain’s tools for monitoring and guiding thought are collectively
known as the ‘executive functions’. The executive functions include attention, working memory, and
processes that switch other cognitive responses on and off according to the needs of the task (Zelazo
et
al
., 1997). The review focused on two of the executive functions most important for decision-making in
contexts of poverty: selective attention and inhibitory control.
Staying focused: selective attention
Selective attention is what enables one to centre on the task at hand, and not to be diverted by
competing tasks or stimuli. It is a measure of the ability to focus one’s mind at will, and to see the
important details of a decision, ignoring less important ones.
Well-functioning selective attention is the opposite of distractibility, and is thus essential to the
successful implementation of decision-making that is in line with one’s goals. Being able to focus on
important tasks is of most obvious utility in the school context, where distractibility is one of the most
common barriers to academic achievement. Beyond education, selective attention is also key to making
good financial decisions and accessing support services – both situations in which large amounts of
information need to be waded through (such as in social service leaflets or application forms) in order to
home in on the key points. Being able to focus on tasks is also critical to good performance at work, just
as it is for making consumer decisions in line with what one needs the most.
Selective attention is measured through the use of one or more cognitive tasks drawn from a set of
measures developed specifically to assess executive functioning, and usually administered in young
samples. These involve asking participants to respond to characteristics of a particular object (or ‘target
stimulus’), which is presented while surrounded by irrelevant objects (or ‘distractor stimuli’). A widely used
example is the flanker task, in which one must press a computer key corresponding to the direction in
which an arrow is pointing, while ignoring a set of distractor arrows flanking the target arrow, which point
in the opposite direction. Another way in which selective attention is assessed is through ratings by
parents or teachers of how focused versus distractible a child is on a daily basis.
Using such measures, a solid set of evidence has built up since 2010 implying a link between selective
attention skills and SES. One of the most robust findings is that a child’s performance on selective
attention tasks at the age of entry into primary school is positively predicted by his or her parents’ SES,
even where the latter is measured as early as when the child was 7 months of age (Bernier
et al
., 2015;
Mezzacappa
et al
., 2011; Razza
et al
., 2010; Ruberry
et al
., 2016; though see Markant
et al
., 2015). A
similar developmental pattern holds later on in life: the higher an adolescent’s family SES as they enter
secondary school, the better their selective attention when they graduate (Boelema
et al
., 2014).
As it is hard to see how one’s cognitive performance while a child could impact one’s earlier levels of SES,
the above evidence implies that there is something about growing up in poor environments that has a
damaging effect on one’s attentional capacities. This allies with evidence that brain structure varies with
SES (Noble
et al.
, 2015), and that experiencing deprivation while very young can hamper brain
development enough to impair executive functioning (Nelson and Sheridan, 2011; see Fell and
Hewstone, 2015). However, most of the above studies are conducted with young people not
experiencing extreme deprivation, but from a range of socioeconomic backgrounds; just one study
14
involved a large number of children whose families could be classified as in poverty. When interpreting
the link between SES and selective attention skills, one must leave open the possibility of a role for stable
cognitive abilities inherited from parents, which might affect both SES and executive functioning. Indeed,
one study showed that when one statistically controlled for mothers’ verbal intelligence, the link between
SES and selective attention was no longer significant (Roze
et al
., 2010).
Holding back: inhibitory control
Success in selective attention tasks involves more than the ability to focus: it also involves the capacity to
resist the urge to set one’s attention elsewhere. This latter skill is part of what is known as inhibitory
control, a process also referred to as ‘cognitive control’ or ‘executive control’. Inhibitory control is the
ability to resist (or ‘inhibit’) the urge to give a response that is facilitated at a particular moment, but is in
fact wrong. An example is found in the Stroop task, one of the most widely used measures of inhibitory
control. Here, participants are asked to press a computer key corresponding to the colour of the ink in
which a word is written. The task is very easy when the word ‘blue’ is written in blue ink, and still easy
when a string of letters, such as ‘XXXX’, is written in blue ink. It becomes more difficult, however, once
the participant sees the word ‘red’ written in blue ink: here, the facilitated response is ‘red’, as that is the
word the participant reads, but the correct response is in fact ‘blue’, because ink colour is what one is
asked to report. Tempting alternative responses, such as pressing ‘red’ in the above example, are known
as ‘prepotent’, and they conflict in the participant’s mind with the ‘target’ response, in the above case,
‘blue’. Thus, inhibitory control tasks measure the extent to which one can resist prepotent responses in
situations of cognitive conflict, in order to produce the target response required by the situation.
Inhibitory control matters for decision-making, firstly, as the inverse of selective attention: only with
strong inhibitory control can one be sure one is not derailed by tempting alternatives, away from the
processing of information or choices in line with one’s goals. It applies beyond cases of concentration,
however, to cases where, for example, using an irregular verb in a foreign language requires resisting the
urge to conjugate the verb as if it followed the pattern of regular verbs. When measured by teachers or
parents through observations, inhibitory control is the ability of a child or young person to control
impulses and to cease engaging in a disruptive behaviour.
Studies of inhibitory control, as with those of executive functioning more generally, have mostly been
conducted among children and young people, in educational settings. As with the other executive
functions, it is already known that inhibitory control is impaired in children who have grown up in
situations of extreme deprivation, because of slower development of the regions of the brain where it is
located (see Fell and Hewstone, 2015). The question considered here was whether there is recent, robust
evidence for a link between inhibitory control skills and income or deprivation across the socioeconomic
spectrum.
The review found evidence for just such a relationship, both when SES and inhibitory control were
measured at the same time points, and when their relationship was investigated over time. Studies with
medium to large samples find that those exposed to longer periods of poverty and more family financial
strain in infancy perform worse on inhibitory control tasks in early childhood (Raver
et al
., 2013;
Rochette and Bernier, 2014). Similarly, low family income during early childhood is linked to poorer
inhibitory control during middle childhood (Hughes
et al.
, 2010; Ruberry
et al
., 2016). Although one study
found no difference in inhibitory control at primary school entry as a function of SES at earlier ages, this
was conducted with a smaller sample size, and with teacher ratings of inhibitory control, rather than
direct assessment (Bernier
et al
., 2015). A similar pattern holds when poverty and inhibitory control are
assessed at the same time: all studies conducted since 2010 (Matte-Gagné and Bernier, 2011; Sarsour
et
al
., 2011; Roy
et al
., 2014), with the exception of two studies with small sample sizes (Colasante
et al
.,
2015; Vandenbroucke
et al
., 2015) find that those higher in SES have better ability to inhibit prepotent
responses.
Moving from primary to secondary school, the review found evidence that SES is positively associated
with performance on inhibitory control tasks. This is observed regardless of whether the two constructs
are measured at the same time (Spielberg
et al
., 2015; though see Micha
et al
., 2010), or whether the
effect of early life SES on later development of inhibitory control skills is considered (Boelema
et al
.,
2014).
15
What is it about SES that might influence the development of children’s inhibitory control abilities? The
link between SES and response inhibition skills seems to be less likely to be found when neighbourhood
deprivation is used as the measure of exposure to poverty (Markovits and Brunet, 2012) than when
family conditions are. Neighbourhood conditions do matter, as it has been found that moving into a high-
income neighbourhood enables improvements in inhibitory skills (Roy
et al
., 2014; Vettenburg
et al
.,
2013), but such wider community factors seem to be mediated through their impact on home and family
conditions. One study found that although the link between SES and inhibitory control is observed at age
6, it is driven not by the family income component of SES, but the education component (Størksen
et al
.,
2015). Digging deeper, it may specifically be the relational aspects of the home environment, such as low
parental responsiveness and family companionship, which explain the ways through which low SES
impairs response inhibition skills in childhood (Bernier
et al
., 2010b). Brain functioning also plays a
mediating role: one study found that compared to high-SES children, low-SES children used up more
neural resources in order to inhibit tempting, non-target responses in an auditory selective attention task
(D’Angiulli
et al
., 2012).
Does SES always predict inhibitory control skills, or are there ‘boundary conditions’ on this relationship,
that is, conditions under which it does and does not apply? Though sample size limits have prevented
thorough investigation of this question, the evidence indicates that the link between SES and response
inhibition is more likely to occur when low-income children are living with only one parent (Sarsour
et al
.,
2011), and is possibly stronger among girls than boys (Spielberg
et al
., 2015; Størksen
et al
., 2015). One
noteworthy study conducted with university students sheds light on situations that might exacerbate the
link between family income and executive functioning. It found that it was only when the potential stigma
of being low in SES was made salient, such as when talking about an academic topic or comparing oneself
to those higher in SES, that those from lower social class backgrounds performed worse than others on
inhibitory control measures (Johnson and Mortimer, 2011).
Thinking, reasoning, and learning
While executive functioning processes are a necessary precursor to good decision-making, higher-level
processes such as reasoning, learning and deliberation play a vital role in the quality of decisions one
makes, particularly more complex ones. These abilities are what come to mind when we think of general
intellectual ability: the capacity to remember information in the long term, and to weigh up competing
perspectives in order to arrive at the best decision. Learning complex concepts, deliberating about one’s
life goals, and taking in lots of information relevant to a particular question, are examples of this kind of
systematic thinking process.
Studies of the link between socioeconomic background and intellectual ability have been mostly
conducted in educational or early childhood settings, and used measures of general intelligence and (in
older samples) academic achievement. Even before reaching school age, it has been found that young
children, regardless of age, ethnicity and gender, perform worse at learning tasks the lower their parents
are in SES (Bornstein and Putnick, 2012; Reid and Ready, 2013; von Stumm, 2012; though see
Kalogrides and Ovink, 2013). As with executive functioning, the socioeconomic conditions of one’s early
childhood environment appear to shape the over-time development of verbal and non-verbal intelligence
as one reaches primary school age (Metcalfe
et al
., 2013; Schoon
et al
., 2010; von Stumm, 2012), up to
age 16 (von Stumm
et al.
, 2013; von Stumm and Plomin, 2015), and even into adulthood (Flensborg-
Madsen and Mortensen, 2014), equally for boys and girls. Heritability in intelligence might again play an
important role in explaining this relationship: although one small study observed that the link between
parental SES and infant intelligence held even when the mother’s intelligence was taken into account
(Betancourt
et al
., 2015), another small study found no link between maternal education and intelligence
at primary school age, once maternal intelligence and was accounted for (Roze
et al
., 2010). As this latter
study also involved taking into account the child’s earlier mental and psychomotor development, it does
not definitively show that it is maternal intelligence that explains the relationship between child
intelligence and maternal SES – a question in need of further study.
A similar pattern emerges in studies of reasoning and learning in the primary school years. A large
number of recent studies find a positive correlation between family SES and performance on intelligence
tests when the two are measured at the same time, usually equally for boys and girls (Flouri
et al
., 2015a;
Holmes and Kiernan, 2013; Hornstra
et al
., 2015; Kalogrides and Ovink, 2013; Marcus Jenkins
et al
.,
2013; Martini and Sénéchal, 2012; Metcalfe
et al
., 2013; Pina
et al
., 2014; Razza
et al
., 2012 Wrulich
et
16
al
., 2013). One study based in the UK also found longitudinal effects of eligibility for free school meals –
an indicator of family-level poverty – at age 7, and fluid intelligence at age 10 (Lütke and Lange-Küttner,
2015). The two studies that did not find a difference in reasoning skills by SES were conducted in small-
to-medium sized Turkish (Beceren, 2010) and African American (Day and Burns, 2011) samples, which
arguably differ in key respects from socioeconomic groups in a UK context.
Through the secondary school years – whether SES is measured through education, income,
neighbourhood deprivation or occupational status – its link with performance on intelligence tests
continues to be reported in several studies (Damian and Roberts, 2015; Kriegbaum and Spinath, 2016;
Rajchert
et al
., 2014; Steinmayr
et al
., 2012). The predictive power of SES holds when one examines
academic achievement, including in cross-national samples (Guttersrud, 2015; Kalaycıoğlu, 2015; Parr
and Bonitz, 2015; Yerdelen-Damar and Pesman, 2013), even controlling for intelligence (and gender: see
Hogan
et al
., 2010).
Finally, two studies looking at socioeconomic differences in intelligence among adults found a similar
positive correlation between the two constructs as is observed in childhood: the higher the SES, the
better the intelligence scores (Betancourt
et al
., 2015; Torres, 2013). Indeed, one study with a large
British sample found that educational attainment in adulthood was predicted by childhood intelligence,
which itself varied by childhood SES (von Stumm
et al.
, 2013) As with earlier life stages, the two studies
that failed to find the relationship between SES and higher-level cognitive skills in young adults used
small samples (Micha
et al
., 2010; Rigoli
et al
., 2012), limiting the extent to which they challenge the bulk
of evidence supporting the relationship.
Though many studies document the link between socioeconomic background and performance in
academic and intelligence tests, few dig deeper to attempt to understand when and why it is found. One
study found that meal quality – specifically, the fact that lower-SES children have less healthy diets –
might play a role in explaining the link between childhood SES and verbal reasoning ability (von Stumm,
2012). Similarly to the case of executive functioning, one set of studies found that performance
disparities between high- and low-income people are exacerbated by tasks that make interpersonal
comparisons salient (Smeding
et al
., 2013). This paper reported that if competitive tasks are reframed in
terms of either collaboration, or mastering a new skill, performance differences by SES disappear
(Smeding
et al
., 2013). Conversely, it appears that using experimental methods to make the stigma of low
SES salient exacerbates SES differences in test performance among college students (John-Henderson
et al
., 2014). It appears that one way of combatting this latter effect is for low-income students to
compare themselves to others who are doing worse than themselves (John-Henderson
et al
., 2014).
Consequences for decision-making
The above three reviews leave no doubt that there is substantial evidence for the link between cognitive
ability and SES, suggestive that not only experiencing poverty, but also having parents who are relatively
low in income or education, makes one susceptible to worse performance on measures of reasoning and
intelligence, and of the executive functions that underpin these skills. Caution must be heeded before
inferring that the relationship runs in this direction, however, as few studies have directly addressed the
possibility that parental differences in cognitive skills influence socioeconomic outcomes, while also,
through genes and parenting style, influencing the cognitive abilities of children.
Four papers have attempted to address this gap by taking an experimental approach to the question of
poverty and cognitive functioning. One study investigated performance differences in an inhibitory
control task and a fluid intelligence task in a sample of Americans who had been asked to consider either
easy or hard financial problems. Though there were no income differences in performance on either task
among those considering the easy financial problem, considering a difficult financial problem caused
those lower in income to perform worse on both measures than those higher in income (Mani
et al
.,
2013). The authors conclude that it is the cognitive strain of thinking about how one can meet one’s
needs without enough money that causes those in poverty to appear as if they are less intelligent than
the rich (Mani
et al
., 2013). Another paper had middle-income participants temporarily feel what it’s like
to be on a low income by randomly assigning them to have very few (versus a lot of) resources in a
computer game. Here, the experimentally ‘poor’ group exhibited worse inhibitory control than the ‘rich’
group, apparently because they were distracted by the concerns of not having enough resources to meet
their needs (Shah
et al
., 2012). The generality of these experimental findings is challenged by a more
17
recent paper, however, which compared performance on inhibitory control, selective attention and
intelligence tasks in two large samples of Americans, who were randomly assigned to be assessed before
versus after their payday (Carvalho
et al
., 2016). Though the financial strain argument implies that one’s
cognition is most disrupted when one is most distracted by considerations of financial need, there were
no performance differences observed in the paid versus unpaid groups, on any task (Carvalho
et al
.,
2016).
A fourth paper examined the influence on cognitive functioning of the temporary experience of low
subjective social status, as experimentally induced by giving people false feedback on where they sat on
the socioeconomic ladder in relation to others in their society (Sheehy-Skeffington
et al
., 2016). Those
who were randomly assigned to be told that they were doing worse than most of their compatriots in
terms of SES made more errors on a task measuring inhibitory control (and on other measures of
executive functioning) than those randomly assigned to receive positive feedback on their relative SES. In
another study in this paper, the low-SES group performed worse on a complex financial decision-making
task, suggestive that the damaging causal influence of perceptions of low societal status on executive
functioning mechanisms has a downstream impact on decisions that depend on the use of such
mechanisms (Sheehy-Skeffington
et al
., 2016). In follow-up studies, the authors found that once the
cognitive tasks were reframed as being linked to later gains in SES, the performance difference
disappeared. They interpreted this finding as indicative that those in the low-SES condition were not
experiencing a deficit in cognitive functioning, but were, rather, distracted by the status threat that the
low-SES feedback induced. This distraction led to worse performance on a cognitive task that seemed
irrelevant to status, but just as good, if not better performance, on the same cognitive task once it was
explicitly linked to a potential status gain (Sheehy-Skeffington
et al
., 2016). Although experimental
evidence of this kind is very helpful in establishing a causal link between the experience of low SES and
thinking processes, it is important to consider how successfully it captures characteristics of life in
poverty as it is actually experienced in the real world.
One notable pattern from the review of this set of processes is the convergence in results across
mechanisms, which makes sense given that good executive functioning is needed in order for one to
perform well on measures of intelligence (Engle
et al
., 1999). Inhibitory control and selective attention
had previously been found to play a role in understanding the link between poverty and academic
achievement (Lipina
et al
., 2005; Mezzacappa, 2004; see also Ardila
et al
,. 2005; Lupien
et al
., 2001; for a
review, see Hackman and Farah, 2009), and one study in this review found they both predicted good
relations with peers and lack of behaviour problems (Razza
et al.
, 2012). The implication is that the poorer
one’s socioeconomic background, the worse one’s likely academic achievement and relations at school,
and subsequent occupational performance and success (Katz and Hartman-Maeir, 1997). The
downstream consequences of the influence of poverty on key thinking processes thus fit into the left-
section of the multidimensional framework introduced in Chapter 2: a case of poorer decision-quality as
a result of one’s socioeconomic position.
Executive functioning skills are linked not only to the ‘slower’ cognitive processes of reasoning and
learning, but also to ‘faster’ cognitive processing, which is based on quick, sometimes unconscious
decision rules (known as ‘heuristics’), and thus which is easily prone to bias (de Bruin
et al
., 2007). While
no papers on this link came up in our review of recent evidence, it has previously been found that low
income and education are associated with greater susceptibility to the use of heuristics and biases in
decision-making (Booij
et al
., 2009; Green
et al
., 1994; Harrison
et al
., 2002; see Lunn and Lyons, 2010).
Such biases can play out most starkly in the economic realm, meaning that not only does poverty make
one more likely to make bad decisions, but also such decisions often impede one’s ability to escape
poverty (see Bertrand
et al.
, 2006). Furthermore, whereas the wealthy may have plenty of time,
technological capacity and social networks that they can use to obtain extra information that might
counter a cognitive bias, those living in poverty are often forced to make decisions in a rushed and
information-poor setting, in which misleading heuristics generally thrive (Lunn and Lyons, 2010). The
systematic study of the influence of poverty on susceptibility to heuristics and biases is thus a research
area ripe for development.
18
5 The influence of poverty on key
behavioural patterns
Though some decisions are the result of thinking, others are the result of a set of often ‘thoughtless’
patterns formed by how one normally behaves. Behavioural habits can be divided into a few general types
of pattern, each of which can influence decision-making and behaviour across multiple domains. For
example, if one is a risk-taker, one is more likely to take risks not only when it comes to spending money,
but also when it comes to engaging in dangerous health behaviours or pushing the social boundaries at
work (see Dahlbäck, 1990). Habits are the product not just of personality, however: they develop over
time, in response to one’s environment. The review focused on the three behavioural patterns that are
most important for decision-making in contexts of poverty: approach orientation, self-regulation and
risk-taking. The report then looks at an aspect of one’s sociocultural environment that can also shape
one’s behaviour in unconscious ways: patterns of how other people behave, and the inferred cues as to
what they expect.
Going for a goal: approach orientation
Approach orientation is a general term used to refer to one’s behavioural stance in relation to one’s
goals. Being approach-oriented means that one is motivated to act in order to achieve one’s goals, and
focused on the rewards on offer if one accomplishes them. The opposite is referred to as being
‘inhibition-oriented’: a state of diminished motivation to pursue one’s goals, which go neglected in favour
of caution and inaction. The more inhibition-oriented one becomes, the more one is focused on threats
in the environment, and the less likely one is to exhibit a proactive response in an ambiguous situation. It
is easy to see how this behavioural stance might lead to decisions that prevent one from moving up in
socioeconomic standing. Indeed, low aspirations and lack of motivation to act in line with them has been
linked with poor academic and occupational achievement, and to difficulty in escaping poverty (e.g. Dalton
et al
., 2016).
Studies of poverty and approach orientation have focused on asking people about either their aspirations,
or how they think about and approach their goals. One distinction frequently studied is that between
intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: being intrinsically motivated means that one takes on a task because of
the rewards one gains out of the action of performing it, such as the choice to study a university subject
in which one is very interested. Extrinsic motivation, on the other hand, is being motivated to perform a
task by what one can obtain at the end, and is exemplified in the decision to choose a university subject
purely because it might guarantee good marks or set one up for a high-paying job. As evidenced by these
examples, most studies of approach orientation are conducted in academic settings.
In primary and secondary school contexts, recent research has found that students with lower social class
backgrounds are less likely than those with higher class backgrounds to report being motivated to master
an academic subject (Berger and Archer, 2015; Bodovski and Youn, 2011, Garriott
et al
., 2013; McCoy
et
al
., 2015), and to display their mastery to others (Berger and Archer, 2015). Low family socioeconomic
status (SES) is also linked in multiple, large studies with having lower educational (Boxer
et al
., 2011;
Bowden and Doughney, 2010; Diamond and Huguley, 2014; Frostick
et al
., 2015; Tynkkynen
et al
.,
2012) and occupational (Gore
et al
., 2015; Schoon, 2014) aspirations, and more uncertain career goals
(Gutman and Schoon, 2012) at secondary school. The three studies that did not find this link were
conducted on small samples that were either solely female (Novakovic and Fouad, 2013) or mostly made
up of American ethnic minorities (Day and Burns, 2011; Perry
et al
., 2012; though see Wolniak and
Rekoutis, 2016). Adolescent career aspirations matter, as, along with ability, they are found to be
important predictors of adult aspirations and income in midlife (Cochran
et al
., 2011; Lee
et al
., 2012),
and to act as an important psychological resource for young people in adverse economic circumstances
(Schoon, 2014).
The link between aspirations and SES is not always straightforward, however: one longitudinal study from
the Netherlands found that although teachers rated low-SES students as less committed than high-SES
students towards the end of primary school, low-SES students themselves reported being just as
19
motivated by, and interested in, academic work as did high-SES students (Hornstra
et al
., 2013). Thus,
there is a need to arbitrate on whether self-reports or behavioural observations are the best indicators of
this form of approach orientation. Another study found that ethnic status might affect the relationship
between low SES and aspirations: students from low-SES families who have migrated internationally
(Bowden and Doughney, 2010) or who have minority ethnic status (Diamond and Huguley, 2014;
Frostick
et al
., 2015) reported higher aspirations than other student with the same socioeconomic
background. Finally, one very large study of young children in the UK found that only among boys was
there a relationship between family poverty status in infancy and occupational aspirations at age 7 (Flouri
et al
., 2015b).
Aside from how high one’s aspirations are, approach orientation also shapes what kinds of incentive
motivate one to work towards them. In young adulthood, it has been found that having a low income,
regardless of one’s gender or ethnic group, means having lower levels of intrinsic motivation when it
comes to study and career choice (Johnson and Mortimer, 2011; Salinas-Jiménez
et al
., 2010). This is in
line with the finding that high school students lower in SES are less likely than those higher in SES to
pursue domain-specific knowledge in line with their interests (Tucker-Drob and Briley, 2012). It also
appears that those lower in SES may have higher extrinsic motivations than those higher in SES (Johnson
and Mortimer, 2011). Two studies, however, find no link between SES and either extrinsic or intrinsic
motivations: one followed a medium-sized sample of young adults in Finland (Sortheix
et al
., 2015), and
the other was conducted with a medium-sized sample of teenagers in Italy (Alivernini and Lucidi, 2011).
How might recent studies enable us to dig deeper, to understand the pathways through which the link
between SES and approach orientation operates? One study found that the influence of neighbourhood
deprivation on diminished academic motivation was at least in part due to the poorer school facilities
provided in low-income neighbourhoods (McCoy
et al
., 2015). Though better school facilities might aid in
keeping up students’ motivation, changing the socioeconomic composition of schools could have the
opposite effect: one longitudinal study found that among low-SES primary school students, and
regardless of age and gender, there was greater improvement in academic motivation only when most
members of one’s class were also low in SES (Hornstra
et al
., 2015).
The above studies of approach orientation in low-income contexts also investigated the downstream
consequences of the observed differences. These were not as straightforward as one might think: though
a large, UK-based, longitudinal study showed that being low in SES meant one was less certain about
one’s career aspirations, this uncertainty actually led to better academic performance in later years and
lower likelihood of dropout, after accounting for differences in parental educational expectations, school
motivation, perceived academic ability and receipt of career advice (Gutman and Schoon, 2012). Thus it
appears that for students who are identical in terms of personal and parental encouragement, those
lower in SES have more open-minded and realistic career aspirations, and end up doing better than their
higher-SES peers who have fixed or highly optimistic aspirations.
The diminished intrinsic career motivation associated with living at or near poverty makes sense in terms
of the low level of exposure one has to jobs that are both rewarding and pay the bills; it is arguably a case
of pragmatic expectations, rather than diminished aspirations. Indeed, in the long run, those on a low
income, who were more extrinsically motivated, ended up earning more than if they had not been
motivated by instrumental concerns (Johnson and Mortimer, 2011). The authors of this study note,
however, that this is not because those who were more extrinsically motivated obtained jobs with higher
salaries, but because they worked more hours in their current job; meanwhile, by virtue of being weakly
intrinsically motivated, they were less likely to have much autonomy or space for self-direction at work
(Johnson and Mortimer, 2011).
Thus, on the one hand, the link between SES and style of approach orientation – i.e. intrinsic versus
extrinsic motivation, and fixed versus uncertain aspirations – is not one of poverty impairing this
psychological mechanism. Rather, it seems that coming from a lower income family equips one with more
pragmatic and realistic expectations, which might be highly adaptive when one is facing poorly equipped
schooling and an unreliable labour market. This fits with the conceptual framework introduced in Chapter
2, and its focus on decision-making in poverty as a product of shifts in psychological focus, as opposed to
psychological deficits (see also Sheehy-Skeffington, in press). On the other hand, if growing up in a low-
income context means that one is more likely to neglect one’s goals and aspirations in general, this can
only have a harmful effect on life outcomes. This is because it makes one less likely to achieve not only
20
learning and career goals, but also goals regarding health, parenting, finances and social relationships,
more broadly.
Focusing on the future: self-regulation
Whereas approach orientation concerns one’s motivation to act in line with one’s goals, self-regulation
refers to which particular goals one privileges over others. Those with strong self-regulatory abilities are
more likely to act in line with long-term, rather than short-term, goals (Fujita, 2011). Doing so entails
being able to resist the impulse to act to achieve an immediate benefit, if it comes at the cost of a future,
greater reward (see Vohs and Baumeister, 2011). Self-regulation thus encompasses the ability to control
impulses, and to delay the gratification of one’s desires until a later time. In economics, this is the
opposite of what is known as ‘future discounting’ – the willingness to pay more to receive something now
than to receive it at some later date. Future discounting is a behavioural manifestation of impatience or
‘present-bias’, implying that one has a restricted time horizon in the decisions one makes.
It is not hard to see why self-regulation and time horizon matter for decision-making in a range of
contexts, not only low-income ones. Most health behaviours require the sacrificing of an immediate
reward, such as a tasty cake or a night on the sofa rather than at the gym, for the sake of a future reward
of better physical health. So, too, when it comes to financial decisions, where being able to resist the
impulse to spend money on a luxury good, for the sake of building up one’s savings account, will pay off
only in the long run. As discussed in Chapter 2 in relation to the temporal dimension of psychological
distance, the notion of present–future trade-offs is a key axis of psychological focus, and characterises
many of the decision-making challenges facing those in poverty. If self-regulation has a particularly high
payoff for those in poverty, it is worth investigating whether at the same time, the experience of poverty
makes one less likely to exhibit it.
Though there are inconsistent results when it comes to the link between delayed gratification and SES in
infants and toddlers (Bernier
et al
., 2010a; Eisenberg
et al
., 2010; Matte-Gagné and Bernier, 2011), a
clearer picture emerges in later years. Two large, longitudinal studies have found that for boys and girls
across ethnic groups, family SES as measured in early childhood is positively correlated with self-
regulatory abilities once a child reaches primary school age (Flouri
et al
., 2014), while also predicting
improvement in self-regulation in the intervening years (Moilanen
et al
., 2010). Living in a family that
struggles to meet its needs, or living in a poor neighbourhood, also means one is more likely to exhibit
problems in behavioural regulation at the same life stage (Piotrowski
et al
., 2013; Roy
et al
., 2014;
Størksen
et al
., 2015; though see Vandenbroucke
et al
., 2015). Conversely, moving into a better
neighbourhood is linked to improvement in self-regulation among children, even when accounting for all
other factors that might be associated with the decision to move (Roy
et al
., 2014).
In the teenage years, lower SES continues to be associated with higher impulsivity and lesser tendency to
delay gratification, as shown by three studies with large samples from three different countries (Damian
and Roberts, 2015; Freeney and O’Çonnell, 2010; Vettenburg
et al
., 2013). Recent research, again using
large samples, has also demonstrated this link over time, such that the lower one’s family income and
education at secondary school entry, the poorer one’s self-regulatory ability at ages 13 and 15, especially
among boys (Evans
et al
., 2012; Khurana
et al
., 2015; Neumann
et al
., 2010; though see Auger
et al
.,
2010).
Evidence from adult samples is usually cross-sectional in nature, and converges on a similar pattern as
the studies with children and teenagers. Young adults who are lower in SES report lesser ability to resist
impulses and greater tendency to procrastinate, and are observed to eat more unhealthy food in the lab,
than those higher in SES, controlling for age and gender (Chow, 2011; Johnson
et al
., 2011). Adults of all
ages and both genders, when questioned in large surveys, report being more impulsive and biased
towards the present, the lower in income and education they are (Lehto
et al
., 2013; Peretti-Watel
et al
.,
2013), and the more they feel deprived compared to their peers (Callan
et al
., 2011).
A small number of studies have attempted to investigate the boundary conditions and mediating
pathways of the link between poverty and self-regulation. The emerging picture seems to be that anxiety
heightens the impact of poverty on self-regulatory failures, as this link is strongest among people who
are generally anxious (Zhao
et al
., 2015), or who are made to think about injustice, mortality or their own
poor performance on a test (Griskevicius
et al
., 2011; Laurin
et al
., 2011). Echoing findings from the
21
literature on executive functioning, another factor that appears to heighten the negative relationship
between low SES and self-regulation is salience of the stigma associated with low social class (Johnson
et
al
., 2011).
Unsurprisingly, given the importance of self-regulation to decision-making, the papers reviewed imply
that its association with SES has negative downstream consequences for those at the bottom of society.
The poorer one is, and thus the less one holds off on current pleasures for the sake of future goals, the
less likely one is to engage in physical exercise (Lehto
et al
., 2013), and the more likely one is to engage
in unhealthy eating, and thus to have higher body mass index in later years (Evans
et al
., 2012). In such
situations, one is also less likely to attempt to quit smoking, and to succeed in each attempt (Peretti-
Watel
et al
., 2013). Present-focused behaviours among low-income groups hamper their later academic
performance (Freeney and O’Connell, 2010), and also make them more susceptible to mental illness
(Flouri
et al
., 2014. Finally, a bias towards the present comes hand-in-hand with the next psychosocial
mechanism – risk-taking – as those with poorer self-regulation skills as a result of low income have been
found to be more likely to gamble (Auger
et al
., 2010; Callan
et al
., 2011), engage in risky sexual
behaviour (Khurana
et al
., 2015), and get involved in violent youth gangs (Vettenburg
et al
., 2013).
Putting things on the line: risk-taking
Risk-taking needs little explanation, defined as the tendency to opt for uncertain outcomes that offer
high possible payoffs, over certain outcomes with lower payoffs. It is also often assumed to be a
behavioural problem associated with low-income groups, whether one reads about the greater incidence
of unsafe sexual practices and drug use among young people in deprived communities, or the popularity
of outlets for gambling on sports and fruit machines in working class areas. Once one looks at recent
empirical papers on the link between SES and risk-taking, however, the relationship appears more
complicated.
In the papers reviewed, risky behaviour was measured either in the form of choices in experimental
gambling tasks, or through observations of behaviours in particular domains. Contrary to popular
assumptions, there are a number of recent studies linking low SES to risk aversion – the tendency to
avoid risks in favour of safer options. One study conducted in Turkey found that controlling for age and
gender, the lower the income, the more likely one is to choose ‘safe’ careers (such as in health and
education) over ‘risky’ careers (such as business: Caner and Okten, 2010). A French study found that
both male and female students in low-income high schools displayed less risky financial preferences than
students in wealthier high schools (Eckel
et al
., 2012), while a study with a large Australian sample found
that lower SES, whether measured at the household or neighbourhood level, was linked to fewer
instances of alcohol-related hazardous behaviour (Livingston, 2014). The one study that looked at risk-
taking in teenage sexual behaviour found no link between this form of risk-taking and income or general
SES (Rew
et al
., 2011).
Thus, studies of those who have had long-term exposure to the bottom of the socioeconomic hierarchy
seem to imply that this experience makes one more cautious as opposed to more risk prone. It seems
that living with few resources leads one to be careful with the resources one has, and more likely to go
for safe options in terms of life choices. Things may change, however, if one’s situation of need becomes
acute, or if one is exposed to the experience of threat, as demonstrated by recent experiments in
different research streams. When participants in one set of studies were presented with cues regarding
an economic recession or thoughts about mortality, it was those with low social class backgrounds who
were the most likely to respond by taking financial risks (Griskevicius
et al
., 2011). In another set of
studies, where the experience of poverty itself was experimentally induced by randomly assigning study
participants to have a very small (versus large) starting budget in a laboratory game, risky economic
behaviour was elicited, and this risk-taking became more frequent as people’s budget increased (Fatas
et
al
., 2011).
A third stream of research assumes that what induces risk-taking is the immediate experience of not
having enough to meet one’s needs, such that the only way to fulfil needs is by taking a gamble. When
participants in these studies were randomly assigned to a game condition in which they had insufficient
resources, they were more likely to make riskier choices in that game than if they started with enough
resources to get by (Mishra and Lalumière, 2010; see also Shah
et al
., 2012). Finally, a fourth set of
findings imply that this sense of not having enough can also be induced through comparison with others,
22
with implications for risk-taking. People who generally feel they have fewer resources than their peers, or
who are experimentally induced to feel as if they have fewer, are more likely to engage in risky decision-
making, including gambling with real earnings (Callan
et al
., 2011; Mishra
et al
., 2014; Mishra and
Novakowski, 2016; though see Carvalho
et al
., 2016). It seems to be the perception that taking a risk is
the only way one can meet one’s needs that is driving these effects (see also Haisley
et al
., 2008). If one’s
needs are met through some other route, or if risk-taking is not seen as a valid option to raise one’s
socioeconomic standing, then risk-taking decreases in frequency again (Mishra
et al
., 2014; Tabri
et al
.,
2015).
In summary, there is no straightforward link between poverty and risk-taking, as conventional wisdom
regarding behaviours associated with poverty seems to assume. Indeed, given that the options for
enacting risk proneness vary widely across the socioeconomic spectrum – from high stakes gambling
through the stock market at the top end, to far lower stakes gambling by buying a lottery ticket at the
bottom – it may be impossible to obtain a definitive view of the relationship of this behavioural tendency
with SES. What does appear to be clear from the above studies is that risk-taking in the context of
perceived resource scarcity is a strategic decision, which depends on one’s enduring experience of
poverty, one’s current pressing needs, and the perceived benefits to be gained (or the perceived lack of
anything to be lost) from taking a risk. Risk-taking is thus another case where the influence of poverty
fits less with a pattern of impairment than of a shift in psychological priorities to address pressing needs.
Doing as others expect: sociocultural norms
Although we have seen how they can be influenced by contextual circumstances such as neighbourhood
deprivation and family resource availability, approach orientation, self-regulation and risk-taking are
three intuitive behavioural tendencies that operate at the individual level. Yet there is another key route
through which behaviours can be elicited without being decided on by conscious thought: social
influence. As we are fundamentally social beings, we strive to behave like others around us whom we
recognise as members of our social group, and we turn to others as teachers of what behaviours work
well, and what is expected of us (Cialdini and Trost, 1998). Behaviourally informed public policy is
increasingly recognising the importance of social norms – that is, patterns of how others in our social
circle behave – in shaping important life decisions (Behavioural Insights Team, 2010). Adding to this, one
prominent claim about ‘suboptimal’ decisions made by low-income groups is that they are a result not of
personal deliberation, but of surrounding cultural contexts that make some ways of behaving seem like
‘what people like us do’ (Lewis, 1996; Moynihan, 1965). Assumptions about unique ‘subcultures’ of
poverty or marginalisation have fallen out of popularity in academic research, but still feature in political
and media representations of recipients of government financial or housing assistance (MacDonald
et al
.,
2014). The review thus searched for recent papers featuring reference to norms and culture alongside
terms indexing poverty and low SES, to capture a snapshot of the strength of the evidence on this
question.
One area that has recently been studied empirically is whether there are differences across the
socioeconomic spectrum in the expectations that parents signal to their children regarding their potential
in the academic and career domain. There is some evidence, including from a very large, cross-sectional
American study and a longitudinal British study, that low-income parents have lower expectations for
their child’s academic achievement than do high-income parents (Gutman and Schoon, 2012; Lawrence,
2015; Perry
et al
., 2012; Sciarra, 2010), an attitude that might be communicated through behaviours
such as engaging in less academic-related play with children (DeFlorio and Beliakoff, 2015). In the
domain of career expectations, however, the one (medium-sized) study that examined the question found
no difference in parental encouragement of career aspirations, through their sharing of work
experiences with their children (Novakovic and Fouad, 2013). Another study of what normative cues may
be passed from parents to children concerned a parenting style described as exhibiting ‘psychological
control’: a large Finnish study found that the lower the SES of parents, the more they expected gratitude
and obedience from their children in return for the sacrifices made in parenting. This appeared to have a
negative influence on parental educational aspirations for their children, in turn predicting children’s
worse academic performance (the latter, in part, because the child’s own aspirations were depressed as a
result: Tynkkynen
et al
., 2012). Finally, one large, UK-based study found that lower-income parents were
more likely to engage in anti-social behaviour, a normative pattern that may send cues to children as to
how it is appropriate to behave (Odgers
et al
., 2012).
23
Moving from the influence of parents to that of the surrounding community, one large American study
found that in low-income, ethnically-mixed neighbourhoods, young people, regardless of age, gender and
ethnic identity, are more likely to be exposed to violence (Warner
et al
., 2011) than in high-income
neighbourhoods, while community members are also less likely than in high-income neighbourhoods to
report crimes to the police (Slocum
et al
., 2010).
Lastly, there are inconsistent findings regarding the influence of peer expectations and behaviours on
young people’s decisions in important domains such as education and health. One very large American
study found an association between parental SES and how much pressure to perform well at school was
communicated from fellow students, such that low-income students experienced the least peer pressure,
regardless of ethnic group membership (Diamond and Huguley, 2014). In contrast, two studies with
predominantly ethnic minority American samples found no negative peer influence associated with low
social class, reporting that the educational expectations and values of peers of low-SES secondary school
students were no lower than those of high-SES students (Perry
et al
., 2012; Boxer
et al
., 2011). There
were just two studies looking at peer norms and expectations in the domain of health. One fairly large
British study found that although the behaviour of peers mattered for young people’s likelihood of
engaging in condom use, low- and high-income students reported no difference in the incidence and
valence of such peer norms (Abraham
et al
., 2011). One large Dutch study found that perceived norms
were a strong predictor of likelihood of smoking, and that such norms were marginally more in favour of
smoking among secondary school students from lower- than higher-income areas (Cremers
et al
., 2014).
Consequences for decision-making
Unlike its relationship with thinking processes, the relationship between poverty and behavioural patterns
is complicated and not yet fully understood. Research is particularly scarce and inconclusive when
considering the behaviours of others, perhaps unsurprising as the studies reviewed were conducted in a
range of settings, and norms are, by their nature, tied to a particular context. Thus, it is not clear how
poverty shapes the normative backdrop that provides indicators as to what kinds of decisions and
behaviour are approved of, and which are poorly valued. Understanding this question is important, as the
evidence is clear that such normative behaviours matter, playing a critical antecedent role in a range of
educational, health and financial behaviours (see Behavioural Insights Team, 2010). To the extent that
poverty triggers a tendency to focus on the proximal social context, as implied by the conceptual model
proposed in this report, it will place a premium, in the process of decision-making, on considerations of
what family and friends would like one to do. This report returns to the implications of findings regarding
parental and community influence when discussing the influence of poverty on the experience of the
social world more generally, in Chapter 7.
Another area in need of more research is the link between poverty and risk-taking. Just as this
association is not clear, so it is also not certain whether risk-taking should be seen as a desirable or
undesirable psychosocial process: it is encouraged in academic and economic domains where one can
afford to fail, but discouraged in the domain of health, and in economic decisions for the most financially
constrained. Some of the above studies found an inverse correlation between risk-taking and another
behavioural mechanism that is more clearly desirable: self-regulation (Auger
et al
., 2010; Callan
et al
.,
2011). Despite the correlation between self-regulation and decisions that lead to positive long-term
outcomes, however, the conceptual framework in Chapter 2 highlights the proximal functions that might
be served by so-called failures in self-regulation. Thus, while spending money rather than saving it, or
engaging in heavy drinking and unhealthy eating, might have damaging financial and health consequences
in the long term, they also offer immediate rewards such as pleasure and social esteem – cherished
goods that can be hard to find in a life at the bottom of the socioeconomic spectrum (see Sheehy-
Skeffington, in press).
Approach orientation is a psychosocial stance that also appears to be unequivocally good. Certainly, to
the extent that poverty entails an attitude of low aspirations and little motivation to succeed, it runs the
risk of embedding a psychological mindset that can entrench one’s poor socioeconomic position. Yet
neglect of goals can be seen as a case of focusing more on the actual than the hypothetical, with both
proximal and distal effects. Although keeping hypothetical ‘better possible selves’ in mind can help to
encourage one to strive towards these future selves, confronting the gap between the actual and the
ideal self can also be demotivating (Oyserman
et al
., 2011). In resource-constrained contexts
24
characterised by environmental instability, it might make more sense to focus on dealing with very real
obstacles and threats, than it does to pin one’s hopes on aspirations that might turn out to be unrealistic.
25
6 The influence of poverty on ways
of navigating life’s challenges
The first two clusters of processes considered concern enduring orientations that one brings with one to
various life contexts. The report now turns to sets of psychosocial mechanisms that are not dispositional
to the same extent, but arise in response to challenges and social situations that one encounters in daily
life. Life in poverty, as in a range of socioeconomic contexts, throws up a range of challenges, from the
struggles to establish one’s identity in adolescence, through the pressure to perform well academically in
order to obtain a stable job, to the stresses of making a relationship work, raising a child and supporting a
family. The resources one harnesses in response to these challenges centre on the notion of belief in the
self: expressing confidence in one’s self-worth in relation to others, one’s ability to succeed in tasks, as
well as faith that one’s actions will have a meaningful impact on future outcomes. Also relevant is what is
broadly known as one’s coping style: whether one tends to respond to a difficult situation by adjusting the
self or the environment, or by denying the existence of the situation altogether. As each of these
mechanisms has an established link to positive decision-making in a range of domains, an important task
is to understand whether there might be an impact of socioeconomic status (SES) on their functioning.
Measuring up: self-appraisal
A primary consideration in determining how one will navigate life’s challenges is what one thinks of
oneself in the first place. Self-appraisal includes self-concept – a person’s image of him- or herself – and
self-esteem – how a person evaluates him- or herself in relation to others. Self-esteem has been heavily
studied in the United States, where one might argue that individualistic cultural values have built in a
culturally-specific link between one’s perceived success in life and one’s worth as a person. Recent
research reflects this national bias, though some studies also investigate the question of the relationship
between SES and self-appraisal in other industrialised countries. Although most studies described below
are cross-sectional rather than longitudinal, they tend to include controls for background variables such
as age, gender and ethnic group membership, in order to understand the relationship of interest as
clearly as possible.
One point of consensus in the literature is that the lower one’s income or education level, the lower
one’s ‘subjective social status’: a construct that is measured by asking people to place themselves on a
ladder representing their society or community, where they are told that those at the top have the best
jobs, income and education, and those at the bottom have the worst (Goodman
et al
., 2001). Researchers
of socioeconomic disparities in health developed this measure in order to document the importance, not
only of objective conditions such as poverty or unemployment status, but also of how people subjectively
construe such conditions. Specifically, they found that it is the influence of SES on one’s self-perceived
rank in society that contributes to socioeconomic health disparities being so stark (Goodman
et al
.,
2001). One recent study from our review supported this claim, reporting in a large US adult sample that
low income and low education are associated with low subjective social status (Seeman
et al
., 2014).
Such subjective construals in turn matter for deeper evaluations of self-worth. One large Canadian study
found that subjective social status was positively correlated with self-esteem in young people, as was
parental education, household income and (inversely) whether one was in a school district classified as in
poverty or found to be very unequal (Quon and McGrath, 2015). The relationship between SES and self-
esteem among adults was also found in two large UK and US studies (Packard
et al
., 2012; Thompson
et
al
., 2012), while another large study linked SES to perceptions of how well one is doing compared to
others in the domains of work, family life and the home.
Among young people, the higher one’s family’s SES, the higher one’s own self-esteem and self-
confidence, as assessed in large samples of secondary school students (Damian and Roberts, 2015; Quon
and McGrath, 2015). Expectations regarding how one will do in school were also found to be related to
SES (Bodovski, 2014), as were whether or not one will proceed to higher education (Sciarra, 2010). One
interesting finding, however, is that (especially female) students in high-income private schools actually
expressed greater envy of their peers than students in equally selective, low-income public schools
26
(Lyman and Luthar, 2014), implying that status anxiety is an issue for adolescents across the
socioeconomic spectrum.
Poor evaluations of the self are also observed among adults low in income (controlling for age and
ethnicity: James and Amato, 2013), and are linked to poor evaluations of what one could achieve, and to
poor self-rated health down the line (Tan and Kraus, 2015). The pathways aren’t definitive, however, as
some boundary conditions point to contexts and resources that might buffer against the negative impact
of SES on self-appraisal. One of these might be cultural, as the one study conducted cross-nationally
showed that though subjective social status predicted well-being in both the United States and Japan,
being unemployed only led to lower subjective social status in the United States (Gnambs
et al
., 2015).
Another boundary condition concerns the belief one has about what social class means for one’s
competence and potential: one set of studies found that only those who held an essentialist view of social
class – in which class identities are based on immutable, biological differences – became more self-
conscious the lower they were in SES. Those with malleable ideas about social class did not experience
the negative consequences of having a low social class identity, presumably because they did not infer
from their current social position any set of fixed qualities or limited possibilities (Tan and Kraus, 2015).
Another route through which one can be protected from the influence of SES on self-evaluation is
through the availability of social or spiritual support. One large American study found that for all ethnic
groups (and especially African Americans), socioeconomic disparities in self-esteem were attenuated
among those who were more religious (Thompson
et al
., 2012). One set of studies conducted in Italy
found that when people were randomly assigned to groups in which they felt relatively low (versus high)
in resources, they in fact responded with more positive evaluations of their own group, with whom they
express sharing a sense of common fate (Moscatelli
et al
., 2014). The implication is that perceiving low
social status in an intergroup context is not as psychologically painful as perceiving it in an inter-individual
context, because one has the group as a resource to maintain positive self-esteem (see Rubin and
Hewstone, 1998). One should, of course, add the caveat here that this study randomly assigned
participants to
ad hoc
groups of varying status; it remains to be seen whether group membership would
fulfil the same buffering function in the case of chronic membership of real stigmatised groups.
Feeling able to perform: self-efficacy
Though self-esteem is an important global construct predictive of well-being and life outcomes, the
construct of self-efficacy is more specific, and thus more potent as a precursor to decision-making. Self-
efficacy is defined as one’s perceived ability to learn new skills and to succeed in the tasks that one sets
oneself (Bandura, 2001). It can be measured as a general, competence-based self-appraisal, or in terms
of specific domains, such as whether one feels efficacious in doing well at school, or in managing one’s
health.
The relationship between poverty and self-efficacy is an already-established one, as public health
scholars have pointed to self-efficacy, or perceived personal control, as one of the most important
psychosocial mediators of the link between SES and health outcomes (Seeman, 2008). The review
nevertheless uncovered several recent papers assessing this link at different life stages, as well as those
investigating it in specific domains.
The pattern emerging from a number of studies, with large samples across diverse national contexts, is
remarkably consistent. Some 13 papers reported that generalised self-efficacy is positively correlated
with SES when people are in primary (Hornstra
et al.
, 2013; Hornstra
et al.
, 2015; Wiederkehr
et al.
,
2015b; Vekiri, 2010) and secondary (Arslan, 2013; Falci, 2011; Kriegbaum and Spinath, 2016; Kwon and
Wickrama, 2014; Moilanen and Shen, 2014; Tikkanen, 2016; Wiederkehr
et al.
, 2015b) school, and at
university (Aguayo
et al.
, 2011; Condon and Holleque, 2013). There were two exceptions: studies
involving an Italian medium-sized sample of high school students (Alivernini and Lucidi, 2011), and a
British medium-sized sample of high school students (McGeown
et al
., 2014), in which no link between
SES and self-efficacy was found. The one longitudinal study conducted found in a large sample that
although one’s parents’ SES at age 1 did not influence one’s self-efficacy at age 13, the latter was
predicted by improvements in family SES and affluence during childhood (Mazur
et al
., 2014). The pattern
continues to adulthood, where 11 papers, some including large, representative samples in different
countries (including the UK and Japan), reported that SES is positively correlated with self-efficacy
(Groffen
et al
., 2012; Kan
et al
., 2015; Moilanen
et al
., 2010, Murray
et al
., 2012; Packard
et al
., 2012;
27
Seeman
et al
., 2014; Schieman and Narisada, 2014; Ward, 2013; Rea, 2016; Sheehy-Skeffington and
Sidanius, 2015; Speer
et al
., 2013). Only one paper failed to find this relationship (Shobe
et al
., 2013),
though this study measured SES only in terms of financial assets, as opposed to including other
components such as education and occupational status.
There is also suggestive evidence that at least part of the relationship between poverty and general self-
efficacy is due to the causal impact of the former on the latter. One set of studies, conducted on small to
medium-sized US samples, randomly assigned participants to have positive versus negative perceptions of
their relative socioeconomic standing in society. Each time, those who were induced to feel low in
subjective social status reported diminished self-efficacy and lesser ability to exhibit control over their life
outcomes (Sheehy-Skeffington and Sidanius, 2015).
Turning to specific domains, it has been found in a number of studies that perceived competence in
academic tasks is lower among students with lower social class backgrounds, as are performance
expectations at school (Karaarslan and Sungur, 2011; Kalaycıoğlu, 2015; Parr and Bonitz, 2015,
Yerdelen-Damar and Pesman, 2013). The pattern holds when efficacy in relation to specific types of
academic subject is considered, from maths and science subjects (Garriott
et al
., 2013; Guttersrud, 2015)
to those based in the creative arts (Karwowski, 2011). The only study that found a result contradicting
this pattern was conducted with a sample of primarily ethnic minority students at an all-girls Catholic
school in the United States (Novakovic and Fouad, 2013), and so is not straightforwardly applicable to the
UK context.
Results are less consistent in the domain of health, where efficacy is measured in relation to one’s
perceived ability to live a healthy lifestyle or to adequately manage a healthcare condition. One large
study reported an association between SES and dietary self-efficacy among American teenagers, such
that those from wealthier families reported being more able to engage in healthy eating (Fahlman
et al
.,
2010). Six studies reported that adults higher in SES expressed greater health-related efficacy, in the
form of perceived ability to engage in adequate physical activity (Lehto
et al
., 2013; Murray and Rogers,
2012), or to manage chronic conditions such as diabetes and heart disease (Murray
et al
., 2012; Lyles
et
al
., 2013; Seligman
et al
., 2010; Vijayaraghavan
et al
., 2011). In contrast, seven studies found no
relationship between SES and health efficacy among young people, whether studied in terms of one’s
approach to general health behaviours (Abraham
et al
., 2011; Hall
et al
., 2016), or to specific cases such
as refraining from smoking (Conner
et al
., 2013; Cremers
et al
., 2014) and engaging in physical activity
(Dzewaltowski
et al
., 2010).
Considering that health behaviours are more specific and varied than school-related behaviours (where
the link with SES is more clear), it seems that SES has its greatest influence on more general and
enduring self-appraisals than on perceived efficacy in specific domains. Given the centrality of education
to measures of SES, there could also be a role played by genetically inherited components of intellectual
ability, both in driving SES and in driving perceived mastery in the academic domain. Overall, the clarity
and robustness of the link between generalised self-efficacy and SES could be due, in part, to the
influence of intelligence on general confidence, and in part to the fact that, almost by definition, domain-
general processes are more psychologically stable than domain-specific ones. Another possibility for
reconciling the findings regarding SES and generalised or academic self-efficacy with those regarding
health efficacy is that health-related behaviours are more subject to additional environmental constraints
unrelated to SES, such as partner support and biological disease risk factors.
This is a question worth resolving, as one point of consensus concerns the importance of self-efficacy for
consequential decision-making in the domains of health, education and beyond. Studies included in this
review found that the low self-efficacy associated with lower-SES groups was linked to worse academic
self-concept and greater perceived ostracism in school (Tikkanen, 2016), and as a result, poorer
academic attainment (Wiederkehr
et al
., 2015b; Yerdelen-Damar and Pesman, 2013). Lower sense of
personal control is also linked to greater chances of relapse into smoking after having quit (Businelle
et
al
., 2013), worse health behaviours (Kwon and Wickrama, 2014), and poorer physical (Greene and
Murdock, 2013; Kan
et al
., 2015; Mazur
et al
., 2014) and mental (Groffen
et al
., 2012; Kan
et al
., 2015)
health outcomes. Even in the domain of civic participation, low SES is associated with lower perceived
efficacy of civic actions, and as a result, lower likelihood of voting (Manganelli
et al
., 2015).
28
Sensing that one’s actions matter: response efficacy
It is one thing to sense that one can carry out actions that one intends, and quite another to be
convinced that one’s actions actually matter for life outcomes. Response efficacy refers to the latter of
these appraisals: one’s judgment of the extent to which external events and personal trajectories are the
product of one’s behaviour, versus of circumstances beyond one’s control. Though some self-report
scales combine components of self-efficacy and response efficacy (e.g., Lachman and Weaver, 1998),
response efficacy is usually measured with the dedicated locus of control scale (Rotter, 1966), on which
the key distinction is whether one sees the source of control of events as primarily coming from within
versus outside of the self. Indeed, it is important to separate the two constructs, as one over-arching
theory of poverty and decision-making states that the most important environmental cue determining
whether one will focus one’s energies on short-term versus long-term goals is the perception of how
much the ultimate life outcome – mortality – is unaffected by one’s actions (Pepper and Nettle, under
review). The report thus turns to findings from the limited set of recent studies that have looked at the
relationship between poverty or SES and perceived response efficacy.
All studies were cross-sectional, and most were conducted on large samples of adults in diverse national
settings. Together, they highlight a consistent pattern of lower SES being associated with a weaker belief
that outcomes are primarily the result of personal actions, and a stronger belief that they are dependent
on external factors such as other people, or mere chance (Bodovski, 2014; Groffen
et al
., 2012; Murray
and Rodgers, 2012; Petersen and Lindstrom, 2010; Prawitz
et al
., 2013; Seeman
et al
., 2014; though see
Greene and Murdock, 2013). The pattern is replicated in studies of young people, whether assessed in
secondary school (Bodovski, 2014) or in their early 20s (Roncancio
et al
., 2012, Kiviruusu
et al
., 2013).
The only study that found those low in SES were more likely to have a higher
internal locus of control
also found that the same group attributed more control to powerful others and to chance than did those
high in SES, a somewhat inconsistent pattern (Grotz
et al
., 2011).
Beyond general appraisals of locus of control, there are also studies of the relationship of SES to ratings
of how much autonomy and self-direction one has in specific contexts. One study reported a cross-
sectional correlation between income, education and perceived control at work (Seeman
et al
., 2014),
and another found that those with lower SES in high school were less likely to experience autonomy in
their jobs 17 years later. As reported in Chapter 5, this latter relationship was primarily driven by those
low in SES having lower intrinsic motivation, and thus being less likely to seek out jobs that feature self-
direction (Johnson and Mortimer, 2011). Domain-specific correlations between SES and autonomy have
also been observed as regards control over one’s finances, over the help one can give to others, and even
over the development of one’s marriage (Seeman
et al
., 2014). Consistent with evidence from the public
health literature, the lower levels of response efficacy reported by those lower in SES are also linked to
worse physical health outcomes in later life (Groffen
et al
., 2012; see also Pepper and Nettle, 2014).
Responding to stress: coping style
The final mechanism that sheds light on how people navigate life’s challenges concerns individual
reactions to situations of pressure and strain, that is, their coping style in response to stress. The study of
coping has played a large role in the development of health psychology, due to the field’s discovery that
stressful situations can be prevented from having negative physical and mental health consequences if
they are responded to with the right mindset. Acknowledging the existence of a threat or stressor, and
being active in responding to it, whether through reappraising it, or adjusting oneself or the surrounding
situation, are found to be healthy, adaptive forms of coping. Maladaptive coping, on the other hand, is
characterised by avoidance or disengagement from the threat (e.g., Billings and Moos, 1981; Endler and
Parker, 1990). Related concepts include resilience, a form of hardiness in the face of stress (see e.g.,
Luthar, 2003), and its converse, a sense of helplessness in response to uncontrollable life events, with its
resulting failure to act at all (Maier and Seligman, 1976).
The papers that met the criteria for this review indicated that children who have experienced more
economic hardship exhibit lower levels of resilience (Bethell
et al
., 2014) and less adaptive functioning as
they reach primary school years, the latter measured in terms of secure attachment, prosocial behaviour,
academic competence and receptive vocabulary (Schofield
et al
., 2011). In the teenage years, it appears
that more active coping over time (and less avoidance coping) is observed among those from higher-SES
families (Glasscock
et al
., 2013; Jaser
et al
., 2012; Walker
et al
., 2015), with one longitudinal study finding
29
that family income played a particularly important role for boys, and family education played the most
important role for girls (Glasscock
et al
., 2013).
The pattern continues through to adulthood, though becomes somewhat less clear-cut. Being low in
income has been found to predict less self-reported coping efficacy in young adults (Danvers
et al
.,
2015), greater negative emotions in postpartum young mothers (Businelle
et al
., 2013), and more self-
reported helplessness and hopelessness (Ree
et al
., 2014). Yet a fairly large study of American adult
parent couples found no effect of income or economic strain on active coping that was focused on
adjusting one’s environment in response to a stressor. The same study reported gender-differentiated
effects on active coping in which one adjusts the self in response to stress: for men, more economic
strain was related to lower self-adjustment, whereas for women, economic strain was linked to greater
self-adjustment (Wadsworth
et al
., 2013).
Another form of effective coping is seeking help from sources outside of the self. The one recent study
that looked at this found no difference in the personal coping strategy of reframing, nor in the external
coping strategies of seeking support from spiritual practices and from one’s family, depending on the
material resources of parents of very young children. This same study did find, however, that parents who
had more of their material needs met were more likely to succeed in obtaining support from friends,
another externally-focused strategy (Maupin
et al
., 2010). It might be fruitful to reconcile this with the
evidence for the link between poverty and social capital (e.g., Cattell, 2001; Knack, 2002).
What might buffer against the negative influence of poverty on coping strategies and abilities? One
strategy that does not seem to differ by SES is known as ‘shifting and persisting’: reframing an obstacle in
positive terms, and sticking with the path towards a desired goal (Chen and Miller, 2012; see also Perry
et
al
., 2012). Two studies in medium-sized samples of Canadian secondary school students found that those
low-SES students who engaged in shift and persist strategies had fewer markers of respiratory or
cardiovascular inflammation than those who did not (Chen
et al
., 2011, 2013). The authors argue that
teaching this coping strategy to low-income students could help protect them against the physiologically
stressful aspects of life at or below the poverty line.
Exploring these and other interventions is critical for protecting those at the bottom of society from
experiencing some of the worst characteristics of life in poverty, namely, the stress and pressure it brings
(see Haushofer and Fehr, 2014). For as well as showing that SES matters for coping strategies, the above
studies also show how important coping is for life outcomes. Downstream consequences of poorer
coping in low-income groups included higher stress levels (Glasscock
et al
., 2013), more signs of
inflammation (Chen
et al
., 2011, 2013), faster heart beat (Danvers
et al
., 2015), poorer diabetes self-
management (Jaser
et al
., 2012), and thus, unsurprisingly, worse physical health (Danvers
et al
., 2015;
Ree
et al
., 2014) and quality of life (Jaser
et al
., 2012, Walker
et al
., 2015). This may have knock-on
effects on loved ones, too, as one study found worse coping among parents was linked to more parental
depressive symptoms, more negative mother-child interactions, and, as a result, greater incidence of
behavioural and mental health problems in children (Wadsworth
et al
., 2013).
Consequences for decision-making
The picture that emerges from this review of recent empirical investigations of the link between poverty
and ways of navigating life’s challenges is, in many ways, a pessimistic one. It implies that the lower one
finds oneself on the socioeconomic ladder, the more poorly one evaluates one’s self worth and one’s
ability to behave in ways that might actually matter for one’s life outcomes. One is thus more likely to
respond to stressors by denying or avoiding the threat encountered, rather than reappraising it to make
it more manageable and thus potentially scalable. In summary, low levels of perceived personal
effectiveness and avoidance-based coping seem to leave little space for behaving in ways that might lead
to long-term improvement in SES. Nevertheless, in line with the conceptual framework guiding this
review, it is worth considering the ways in which such response orientations might facilitate decisions that
are adaptive in a proximal sense.
Though self-esteem is often taken for granted as a psychological good in American contexts, there is
good reason to believe that holding an inflated image of the self is maladaptive not only for social
relational purposes, but also as a guide to behaviour. First, research into the psychology of social class
suggests that in working-class contexts, expressions of self-praise, rather than being seen as a necessary
30
part of self-promotion and career success, may in fact be damaging to one’s reputation, as they violate
cultural expectations regarding interdependence, humility and solidarity (for a review, see Stephens
et al
.,
2014). If this is the case, then low-SES students with more modest self-concepts may flounder in
universities and workplaces only to the extent that such spaces are structured in terms of middle-class
cultural expectations focused on self-advancement (Stephens
et al
., 2012). To the extent that
experiencing relative deprivation in resources facilitates a greater focus on one’s proximal social group, as
implied by the conceptual framework and shown by one of the above studies (Moscatelli
et al
., 2014),
adaptation to local norms regarding expressions of self-esteem makes rational sense. Combining modest
self-appraisal with limits on self-efficacy and response efficacy might also be helpful for making realistic
plans to meet one’s goals, as the failure to acknowledge real constraints is known to be damaging both to
effectiveness and to well-being (Seeman, 1991).
Focusing further on self-efficacy, research in social psychology indicates that being deprived of a sense
of control triggers anxious efforts to regain control (e.g., Pittman and D’Agostino, 1989; Rothbaum
et al
.,
1982). One means of achieving this might be a decision to stay focused on local geographic spaces that
seem navigable and manageable – a proximal shift on the spatial dimension of psychological distance,
which again achieves important contextual goals – even if it means one misses out on opportunities in
other neighbourhoods or workplaces. Regarding response efficacy, if those living in contexts of poverty
in fact hold accurate perceptions of the extent to which their life outcomes are primarily under the
control of external forces, then it is even adaptive in an evolutionary sense for them to focus on short-
term goals, such as gaining status in local hierarchies, rather than long-term goals, such as maintaining
physical fitness into old age (Pepper and Nettle, 2014).
Finally, the influence of poverty on coping style may be a product both of adaptive regulation, and of an
impairment in psychological resources. Given the evidence from Chapter 4 for the disruptive cognitive
impact of poverty, it is understandable why the cognitively demanding coping strategies of self-
adjustment and problem-reframing may be too much to ask of a brain dealing with multiple threats of
deprivation. Indeed, given that all cognitive work takes more effort in resource-constrained conditions
(Mullainathan and Shafir, 2013), a more plausible and sensible use of one’s limited efforts might be in
blocking out salient threats for the moment, in order to get on with the daily grind of putting food on the
table and paying the most pressing bills.
In summary, the evidence under this cluster of psychosocial mechanisms implies that there are
comprehensible, if not yet definitive, routes through which low SES might drain a person’s capacity to
respond to challenges in the most ultimately successful ways. Understanding why such pathways make
sense in the context of poverty, and the functionality of the decisions that might ensue, will help in
designing interventions that can empower those at the bottom of society in a truly sustainable and
effective way. Rather than focusing on improving an individual’s sense of self-efficacy or response
efficacy, for example, one might focus on objectively increasing the autonomy they have over life
choices, such as which school to attend and what hours to work.
31
7 The influence of poverty on
appraisals of the social world
As highlighted throughout this report, the experience of poverty is more than one of individual financial
need, and does not occur in a social vacuum. Rather, it is more often a situation of relative need, which is
experienced by families and communities collectively, and has interpersonal and intergroup
consequences. Similarly, decision-making is the product not only of individual psychological resources,
but also of representations and goals that emerge at the collective level, through processes of social
identification and the setting of norms, and the collective construction of values regarding how to
behave. Whereas Chapter 5 considered how the behaviours of others might unconsciously influence
one’s decision-making, this chapter focuses on how one consciously thinks about the social world in
which one is embedded. This involves, first, a consideration of who one is socially: the groups to which
one belongs and the groups from which one is excluded. Next, it involves considering how one interprets
the actions of others, and how one predicts they are likely to behave. These factors feed into one’s own
decisions about how to interact with other people, and about what values one thinks others should share.
This chapter considers each of these mechanisms of social appraisal in turn.
Feeling part of the group: perceived inclusion
Perceived social inclusion is a general heading used to denote a subjective sense of belonging to a social
group, which varies according to the group considered. To the extent that social exclusion forms part of
the definition of poverty (Goulden and D’Arcy, 2014), it is important to consider whether there is, in fact,
an association between one’s socioeconomic position and one’s reported experiences of inclusion and
exclusion in daily life. This can be measured by asking people to report the extent to which they feel they
belong in a particular context (e.g. school, the workplace) or by assessing people’s sense of
connectedness to a salient social identity (e.g. ‘working class’). Perceived inclusion in a particular social
group matters not only because of its established relationship with well-being (e.g., Umberson
et al
.,
1996), but also because it will shape how one treats other members of that group.
The small number of studies looking at this topic tends to corroborate the association between poverty
and a subjective sense of social exclusion. At a young age, children from low-income families are slower
than those from higher-income families to grow in their sense of belonging to their primary school as
they proceed through it (Hornstra
et al
., 2013). Sense of connection to secondary school is also
correlated with socioeconomic status (SES), as two medium-sized studies found that those with better-
educated parents reported a stronger feeling of belonging to their school than those with less-educated
parents (Boxer
et al
., 2011; Tikkanen, 2016). Much of this seems to be occurring through relationships
with peers: two large studies of Norwegian teenagers reported that the lower the SES of one’s family, the
worse one’s relationship with one’s peers (Veland
et al
., 2015), and the less one sees oneself to be
accepted by them (Sletten, 2011). The first of these studies even found a greater likelihood of being
bullied, the lower one’s SES background (Veland
et al
., 2015), an observation supplemented by the finding
that adolescents in low-income American secondary schools reported experiencing more sexual
harassment from peers than did those in middle- or high-income secondary schools (Lyman and Luthar,
2014). One study did not report findings consistent with the above pattern, finding no link between SES
and self-reported sense of ostracism in a medium-sized sample of American high school students
(Garriott
et al
., 2013).
Beyond the school years, there is some evidence that the lower one’s SES in adulthood, the lower one’s
sense of inclusion in, or attachment to, one’s surrounding community. One study found that lower-
income mothers reported having less social support than higher-income mothers (Harknett and Hartnett,
2011), and another found that community volunteers (especially males) who were lower in SES reported
feeling a weaker sense of belonging to their community than did those higher in SES (Speer
et al
., 2013).
Finally, in a study with large samples from both the United States and Japan (Gnambs
et al
., 2015), being
unemployed was associated with a decreased sense of having an important role to play in a collective.
32
The one experimental study that looked at perceived inclusion and SES involved randomly assigning
participants to be in a group that had either more or less money than another social group, thus varying
both absolute wealth and relative wealth. As reported in Chapter 6, the authors found that those placed
into the low-income group had a greater sense of a shared or common fate with their fellow group
members, though here the sense of exclusion from other groups was not measured (Moscatelli
et al
.,
2014). Considering this finding alongside the others reported above, it is possible that being low in
income leads one to feel less included in society at large and the institutions representing it (such as
schools), but perhaps to feel a stronger sense of connection to others also sitting at the bottom of the
socioeconomic spectrum.
Inferring about others: social perception
Whereas inclusion touches on the group to which one thinks one belongs, social perception refers to
how one interprets the behaviours of others, both within and beyond the group. One important
component of this is attribution: the process of making inferences from observing a person’s behaviour
about what their intentions or traits might be. Social perceptions matter for decision-making, as the ways
one interacts with others and engages with social groups and institutions will depend on how one expects
members of those groups and institutions to behave. Most of the limited set of recent studies looking at
this mechanism centre on the correlation between individual SES and the extent to which one has a
general trusting orientation, versus a sense that people in general are selfish and untrustworthy. Another
set of studies look at the link between one’s own socioeconomic standing and the attributions one makes
about how socioeconomic positions are arrived at in the first place – notions that feed into stereotypes
about social class. All studies were conducted in adult samples, in either the United States or Europe.
Most of the recent evidence on social trust is obtained from large European samples, and converges on
the finding that regardless of age, gender and even political ideology, the lower one’s SES, the lesser is
one’s willingness to report that others are generally trustworthy (Petersen and Lindström, 2010;
Trautmann
et al
., 2013). This is corroborated by two longitudinal studies, one of which found in a UK
sample that the SES of one’s parents at age 11, along with one’s own SES at age 33, predicted one’s level
of general social trust at age 33. The other reported, in two large US and UK samples, that increases in
income over time were associated with increases in social trust over time, controlling for age, gender and
life satisfaction (Brandt
et al
., 2014). Higher social trust might be expected to come with lower
Machiavellianism – a personality trait indexing one’s judgment that everyone is ‘only out for themselves’,
such that it is fine to exploit them for one’s own benefit (Paulhus and Williams, 2002). However, the one
study that measured Machiavellianism along with income in a medium-sized US sample found no
correlation between the two (Chen and Tang, 2013). Related to the issue of social trust, one study on a
medium-sized female American college sample found that low SES was linked with greater detection of
hostility in a higher-SES interaction partner (controlling for ethnicity: Kraus
et al.
, 2011). The same paper
also reported in a separate study that lower social class rank, whether measured through objective SES or
induced through the experimental shifting of subjective perceptions, is associated with more hostile
interpretations of a fictitious story vignette, across age and ethnic groups (Kraus
et al.
, 2011).
Moving from general trust to stereotypes about specific social groups, one key dimension of social
perceptions is the extent to which one employs individualistic versus structural explanations for the
socioeconomic position of others. Individualistic explanations attribute people’s socioeconomic outcomes
to their personal characteristics, such as intelligence or hard work. Structural explanations, on the other
hand, put explanatory weight on wider societal factors in understanding the reasons for differences in
socioeconomic outcomes, such as the state of the economy, or discrimination enacted by institutions. It
appears that those higher in SES are more likely than those lower in SES to make individualistic
attributions in this domain, claiming that people’s financial success is mostly down to merit (Tan and
Kraus, 2015; Wiederkehr
et al
., 2015a). One of these studies found that once participants were thinking
about possible failure, however, it was those lower in SES who were more likely to endorse individualistic
attributions (Wiederkehr
et al
., 2015a). Though this might be seen as a motivational strategy for low-
income students, such endorsement of individualistic beliefs regarding success can exacerbate the
negative impact of being low in SES on well-being (Tan and Kraus, 2015). The one large, American study
that complicates this picture found that those low in income and education made not only less
individualistic attributions for poverty, but also less structural attributions, regardless of gender and ethnic
identity (Merolla
et al
., 2011). The authors put this pattern down to a form of ‘dual consciousness’, arising
from exposure to social conditions that simultaneously foster positive and negative attributions regarding
33
the behaviour of those who are in poverty; a more simple explanation, however, is that those who are
lower in SES are more likely to agree to any kind of explanation presented to them in a questionnaire – a
form of acquiescence bias that we already know is inversely correlated with education level (Meisenberg
and Williams, 2008).
Though the studies of general social trust fail to distinguish between perceptions of people inside one’s
social group and perceptions of non-group members, the studies of explanations for SES outcomes imply
that those lower in social class make more generous attributions about other people also low in social
class, by saying their poor economic position was not arrived at through any fault of their own. This
touches on the notion of ‘ingroup bias’, or the extent to which one has more positive social evaluations of
members of one’s own social group than of non-members. Three experimental studies addressed this
question in the case of people randomly assigned to the temporary experience of low or high SES. In one
study, those who were assigned to a group associated with low status traits (as opposed to one associated
with high status traits) were more likely to attribute warmth in judgements of their ingroup, but more
likely to attribute competence in judgements of the other group (Capozza
et al
., 2012). There is also the
experimental study reported earlier, in which being assigned to a low-income group led to better
evaluations of one’s own group, perhaps driven by participants’ greater sense that they shared a common
fate with other low-status group members (Moscatelli
et al
., 2014). In a similar vein, those randomly
assigned to feel relatively deprived have been found to be less judgmental of a fictitious criminal who was
described as low in social class, than they were of one high in social class (Sharma
et al
., 2014). The
emerging picture from these studies is that though being low in SES does not prevent one from
recognising the greater achievement, and possibly skills, of those higher in SES, it may nevertheless mean
that one has more warm feelings and sense of connectedness towards fellow members of one’s own
social class, than one would if one were higher in SES. The report considers what the implications of this
might be for social interactions in the next section.
Interacting with others: interpersonal warmth
Once one has a notion of group membership and a theory of why people behave the way they do, an
important decision is how warm or cold to make one’s interactions with others. Interpersonal warmth is
used here as an umbrella term, to refer to the extent to which one behaves positively, cooperatively and
agreeably with others, versus negatively, competitively and aggressively. Interpersonal warmth is
underpinned by high levels of empathy and compassion, and the ability to tune into the feelings of others.
This matters not only for the success of one’s social relationships, but also for how well one can build up
social capital and navigate the politics of one’s workplace. As revealed by the substantial set of evidence
described below, interpersonal warmth also manifests in the domain of parenting, where displaying
warmth in one’s interactions with children is contrasted with harsh or negative parenting. Early indicators
of interpersonal warmth are often measured in the recording of so-called ‘internalising’ or ‘externalising’
behavioural problems in young people. Internalising problems centre on social withdrawal, whereas
problematic externalising involves engaging in aggressive or intimidating interactions with others.
When it comes to the traits of empathy and compassion for others, the relationship between SES and
interpersonal warmth is not fully clear. Medium-to-large-sized studies from three national contexts
(Spain, USA and Turkey) report a positive correlation between SES and the tendency to empathise with
others (Sánchez-Pérez
et al
., 2014), as well as to correctly detect their emotional states (Raver
et al
.,
2015). These studies find that lower levels of empathy observed among low-income groups are linked to
poorer emotional regulation (Raver
et al
., 2015), and worse interpersonal skills (Beceren, 2010) as
children grow up. There is also a set of research that challenges this pattern, involving observations from
small, predominantly ethnic minority, US samples. Two such studies reported greater trait compassion
and empathic concern among those lower in social class, taking into account gender, ethnic identity, and
other traits such as spirituality and optimism (Stellar
et al
., 2012). Another study found no SES
differences in the tendency to take on positive emotions of an interaction partner (through a process
known as ‘emotion contagion’) in a female student sample, but did find that those lower in SES in the
interaction were more likely to track and mimic their partner’s level of hostility (Kraus
et al.
, 2011).
Moving from empathy to prosocial behaviour, a further four studies observed greater generosity and
helping among those low in either subjective social status (again controlling for age, gender and, this time,
religiosity) or income (Piff
et al
., 2010). One large cross-sectional American study also found a pairing of
low SES and agreeableness, controlling for age, gender and ethnicity (Chapman
et al
., 2010). Finally,
when subjective social status was experimentally manipulated among young adult participants, there was
34
again a link observed between low perceived SES and higher interpersonal warmth, this time in the form
of generous charitable donations (Piff
et al
., 2010).
The picture is more consistent once one moves from mild manifestations of low interpersonal warmth
(such as lack of compassion and generosity), to more severe manifestations, such as aggression and
hostility. As young as preschool age, large studies document that more aggressive behaviour is observed
in children from low-income families or those in poverty, compared to those from middle- and high-
income families (Browne
et al
., 2012; Zilanawala and Pilkauskas, 2011; Kalogrides and Ovink, 2013). Early
childhood aggression among children from low-income families and neighbourhoods is also associated
with greater aggression and behavioural problems exhibited in later years, regardless of gender, age,
ethnic identity, and even maternal education (Holmes and Kiernan, 2013; McCoy
et al
., 2015).
Once children reach primary school, a number of cross-sectional and longitudinal studies from a range of
countries find that regardless of age and gender, those from low-SES backgrounds are not only less co-
operative and sympathetic, but also more aggressive, than those from higher-SES backgrounds (Bassett
et al
., 2012; Baydar and Akcinar, 2015; Bodovski and Youn, 2011; Colasante
et al
., 2015; Razza
et al
.,
2010). Two studies find that lower-income students at this age are more likely to exhibit the aggression-
focused externalising behavioural problems (Denham
et al
., 2013; Jin
et al
., 2010), though a third, larger
study found no such link between SES and behavioural problems in primary school (Véronneau
et al
.,
2015). Changes over time in this age group are harder to predict: one large study found no link between
parental occupational status and the development of aggressiveness throughout primary school (Malti
et
al
., 2013), while another found that SES-based differences in aggression at age 3 did not continue
through to age 6 (Metcalfe
et al
., 2013).
At secondary school age, multiple studies again find higher aggressiveness, and lower sociability, among
male and female adolescents from lower- (versus higher-) income groups (Conger
et al
., 2015; Damian
and Roberts, 2015; Quon and McGrath, 2015; Uz Bas and Yurdabakan, 2012; though see Santiago
et al
.,
2011). Even when poverty is measured at the neighbourhood level, it is found to predict anti-social
behaviour among those in this age group in the UK (Neumann
et al
., 2010). The other context-level
factor that seems to matter is the level of inequality of a secondary school, which was correlated with
aggressive behaviours in one large Canadian study (Quon and McGrath, 2015). The above-mentioned
evidence concerning perception and spread of hostility in interactions and vignettes (Kraus
et al.
, 2011)
implies that being low in SES means exhibiting greater readiness for and attunement to aggression in
one’s social context in early-to-middle adulthood (see Ellis
et al.
, 2012). A larger longitudinal study found
that SES differences in aggression displayed during the teenage years continued through to adulthood
(Conger
et al
., 2015), and another found that they are predictive of later parenting behaviours such as
having children young and not being present for their birth (Serbin
et al
., 2011).
Indeed, once one reaches adulthood, the primary means through which differences in interpersonal
warmth versus hostility are studied is through parenting behaviour. Though one study found no
difference in mother’s self-reported parenting style by SES (Browne
et al
., 2012), a number of large
observational studies, across national contexts, find that the higher the family SES or the mother’s
education, the more responsive a mother is (Baydar and Akcinar, 2015), and the more warm and the less
harsh her parenting style (Browne
et al
., 2012; Henninger and Gross, 2016; Moilanen
et al
., 2010;
Newland
et al
., 2013; Odgers
et al
., 2012; Razza
et al
., 2010; see also Ellingsen
et al
., 2014). The four
studies that did not find this difference in parenting style by SES were again conducted with smaller,
sometimes atypical, samples than those that did find the link (Smith, 2010; Szepsenwol
et al
., 2015;
Wadsworth
et al
., 2013). The relationship persists when one moves from researcher observations of
parenting style to reports given by children (Kwon and Wickrama, 2014; Ritchie and Buchanan, 2010). It
is also observed with fathers’ parenting styles: one large study found that paternal engagement was lower
among low-SES American families (Bronte-Tinkew
et al
., 2010), while two others reported higher levels
of father aggression with lower income (Conger
et al
., 2015; Raver
et al
., 2015). Lower family income is
also linked with less co-operative parenting (Schoppe-Sullivan and Mangelsdorf, 2013) and lower
supportiveness between parents (Bronte-Tinkew
et al
., 2010). Young parents who themselves grew up in
low-SES households end up engaging in more negative parenting once they have children (Trentacosta
et al
., 2010), and exhibiting lower emotional stability and agreeableness (Schofield
et al
., 2011).
Thus, it seems that the greater one’s exposure to poverty or low SES, the more harsh and less responsive
the parenting one is likely to receive from both caregivers. Unsurprisingly, this has negative downstream
35
consequences for strength of parental bonding with children (Veland
et al
., 2015; though see Novakovic
and Fouad, 2013). It also seems to result in poorer child vocabulary and interpersonal behaviours (Baydar
and Akcinar, 2015; Razza
et al.
, 2010, 2012). Stricter parental discipline (Hakulinen
et al
., 2013; Smith,
2010) and colder parenting style (Odgers
et al
., 2012) observed in low-SES families in early childhood
were also linked to increased anti-social behaviour and delinquency in late childhood (Hyde
et al
., 2010),
including in one large UK-based longitudinal study (Odgers
et al
., 2012). Finally, hostility and anger in
adulthood appear to be predicted by SES differences in parenting styles experienced while young,
controlling for age and sex (Conger
et al
., 2015). Yet there are also complications in the relationship
between parenting style and later outcomes: though parenting beliefs are commonly transferred from
low-SES parents to their children, one study that tested found no evidence of a difference in prevalence
of positive versus negative parenting beliefs by SES (as measured by household occupational status:
Erzinger and Steiger, 2014). Another, small Israeli study found that it was children of higher-SES parents
who reported greater frustration than those of lower-SES parents, because their parents were more
critical and demanding of them (Grebelsky-Lichtman, 2014). A final study found that lower-SES children
in the US reported feeling less alienation from their parents despite the fact that they received more
criticism from them (Lyman and Luthar, 2014).
Few studies shed light on the mediating mechanisms of the link between SES, parenting style and
interpersonal aggression. It was found that transitioning into parenthood involves more negative
emotions for low-SES (compared to high-SES) mothers (Schoppe-Sullivan and Mangelsdorf, 2013), likely
because of financial pressure (Newland
et al
., 2013), with implications for mental ill-health (Newland
et al
.,
2013). Meanwhile, one study suggested that the link between neighbourhood deprivation and
behavioural problems is mediated by poorer student–teacher interactions (McCoy
et al
., 2015). In terms
of compounding factors, once low income is combined with stress and/or family conflict, the use of harsh
discipline by parents increases even further (Pereira
et al
., 2015).
Deciding what matters: values and needs
Having considered social perceptions and behaviour, a final key component of one’s orientation to the
social world consists of judgements of what is ultimately the right way to behave. Values articulate what is
important to a person in their life and motivate the choices that they make. They have been summarised
into key dimensions shown to replicate across cultures, one of which taps whether one gives high
importance to self-enhancement (through power and achievement) versus to ‘transcending’ the self,
through humanity and benevolence (Schwartz, 1992). Values develop as a joint product of the individual’s
needs, traits, temperament, culture, socialisation and personal experiences (Bardi and Goodwin, 2011),
and they have an overarching effect on perceptions, attitudes and behaviour (Bardi
et al
., 2008). As such,
values often reflect social conventions and are projected into one’s social circle as a prescription for how
others ought to behave. Studying values in the context of poverty is important not only as a way of
addressing the once-influential tradition of study of the so-called ‘culture of poverty’, in which low-
income American families were described as valuing welfare dependency and idleness over hard work and
success (Lewis, 1996; Moynihan, 1965). Rather, there is a more recent empirical consensus that personal
values matter for important life decisions, such as how much to invest in education, which careers to
aspire toward and how to treat other people. As a result, it is worth asking whether recent research
documents any associations between SES and values of different kinds.
Some values are unsurprisingly correlated with education, such as whether health should be an important
consideration in food choice for children (Zarnowiecki
et al.
, 2012), and the importance of schooling
(Boxer
et al.
, 2011). Interestingly, what underlay the observed link between less-educated families and
lower valuation of school by adolescents in this study was the fact that school was pitted against the
importance of one’s peers, which were rated more highly the lower participants’ family SES (Boxer
et al.
,
2011; see also Steinmayr
et al.
, 2012). Indeed, one might take a second look at the association between
SES and educational and career aspirations, reported in Chapter 5 (see also Beal and Crockett, 2013), by
considering the possibility that such aspirations might be down-regulated in favour of social goals.
Consistent with this, individual preferences for self-enhancement values (including personal achievement)
are found, across cultures, to be negatively associated with preference for self-transcendence values,
such as caring for others (see Bardi and Schwartz, 2003).
It is thus worth exploring whether the lower one’s social class background, the more one values others
over the self, as predicted by the line of research into the psychology of social class (see Piff
et al
., 2010;
36
Stephens
et al
., 2014). Indeed, three studies reviewed for this report found that those lower in SES
exhibited greater self-transcendent values, whether measured directly with a values scale (Rea, 2016),
through the self-reported importance of benevolence (Trail and Karney, 2012), or through prosocial
behaviours in experimental games (Piff
et al
., 2010). Furthermore, a German study of ratings of linguistic
terms found that low-income cultural contexts appear to feature more positive evaluations of concepts
such as intimacy, and less positive evaluations of concepts such as achievement, than high-income
cultural contexts (Ambrasat
et al
., 2014).
Though those living in circumstances of, or close to, poverty value personal achievement and influence
less than those far from poverty (Rea, 2016), this does not mean that they are neglect material goals. In
fact, being lower in SES is associated with a greater focus on physical and economic needs, as manifested
in endorsement of materialistic values (Petersen and Lindström, 2010) and a focus on security as a factor
in job choice (Sortheix
et al
., 2015). Meanwhile, high-SES counterparts report greater adherence to
post-materialistic values such as autonomy and self-expression (Petersen and Lindström, 2010), a finding
reminiscent of their greater intrinsic motivations when approaching life goals (see Chapter 5).
Complicating this picture, one medium-sized French study found no link between parental education and
whether one valued autonomy for one’s children (Tazouti and Jarlégan, 2014). Another (medium-sized,
UK) study found that inter-generational improvement in SES predicted an increase in materialistic values
in the later generation, though here the increase was not in caring for basic material needs, but about
projecting status through the acquisition of goods (Carr and Vignoles, 2011).
Another set of values that might play a similar role, for low-income groups, as do power and achievement
values for high-income groups, concerns conformity and tradition. One study found that lower-SES
parents valued conformity more in their children than did high-SES parents (Tazouti and Jarlégan, 2014).
Other studies found that tradition (Trail and Karney, 2012) and conformity (Rea, 2016) were more
generally valued among low-, compared to high-SES participants.
No recent studies have looked at the downstream consequences of SES differences in values, with one
exception. This found that the greater value placed by those lower in SES on security in career choices
resulted in lesser likelihood of unemployment years later (Sortheix
et al
., 2015). Yet again, what looks like
a less ambitious or success-oriented mindset turns out to be a possibly adaptive one for those living a life
at risk of poverty.
Consequences for decision-making
The study of poverty and social appraisals is complicated both by the complexity of phenomena, and by
the many ways in which the relationships involved might play out. Given the importance of group
membership, one perspective that stands out as missing from the above-reviewed research is an explicit
consideration of whether or not the social targets of one’s attributions, behaviours or values are seen as
members of one’s social identity group. Indeed, perhaps one way of reconciling the seemingly
contradictory findings regarding the relationship between SES and a prosocial orientation is to posit, first,
that life on little or no income forces one to depend on immediate others in one’s social context. Such
‘ingroup members’ should be perceived as particularly vital, for one both to support and to be supported
by – a form of interdependence that leads to the emergence of a construal of the self as interwoven with
and responsible to others (see Stephens
et al
., 2014; see also Day
et al
., 1999, for an anthropological
account of social closeness enforced by societal marginalisation). It is this notion of interdependence
which underpins theories of social class that link growing up in working-class contexts to more
collectively-oriented notions of agency, and with it, a greater valuing of doing as others do, and of
helping out those in need (Stephens
et al
., 2011; Piff
et al
., 2010).
However, a shift towards the proximal on the social dimension of psychological distance (see Chapter 2),
along with the importance of group distinctiveness in establishing social identity (see Tajfel and Turner,
1979), should predict that this co-operative, collective self extends no further than the boundaries of
one’s salient ingroup in a particular context, whether that be one’s family, one’s gang of friends, or one’s
entire community and social class. Thus, while lower SES might mean greater willingness to help socially
close others at a cost to the self, it could also entail a greater hostility, or mistrust, towards those who are
not classified as fitting into one’s social group. Such a prediction, along with the link between poverty and
perceived exclusion (e.g., Gnambs
et al
., 2015), seems to make sense of observations that young people
from poorer neighbourhoods exhibit greater hostility towards strangers and authorities (e.g., Kalogrides
37
and Ovink, 2013), yet stronger commitment to peer groups such as gangs (Thornberry, 2003), and
greater benevolence to those in need (Piff
et al
., 2010). Indeed, there is reason to believe that aggressive
behaviour exhibited even among friends leads to increases in social status at the local level, particularly
for young men (Ellis
et al.
, 2012). Though proximal benefits of such a social stance might include stronger
and more supportive relationships among family and friends, possible negative downstream consequences
are that those with lower-SES backgrounds are more likely to feel alienated from society, and less likely
to make new friends at university, or to form wide social networks in the workplace.
Without further study, it is impossible to know with certainty whether group membership acts as a
boundary condition for the observed link between lower social class and more positive social perceptions
and behaviours. One association it does not seem well-fit to explain is that between SES background and
parenting styles, as children are arguably the most socially close of all, and yet are the target of harsh
parenting by parents experiencing lower incomes (e.g. Browne
et al
., 2012), with knock-on effects for
child aggression (Hyde
et al
., 2010). One possibility is that negative and harsh parenting styles do not, in
fact, fit the values dimension that seems most relevant to interpersonal warmth: whether one values
others (self-transcendence) over the self (self-enhancement). Rather, they may be a manifestation of
SES differences in the other key dimension of values: a greater valuing of conformity and tradition over
autonomy and openness to novelty (see Rea, 2016; Tazouti and Jarlégan, 2014). Strict parenting styles
and a greater focus on developing ‘toughness’ might be part of a sociocultural norm adopted by low-SES
parents because it is what is needed to survive in the harsh environments characterising many poor and
working class contexts (see Ellis
et al.
, 2012; Frankenhuis
et al.
, 2016). Seen through this light, harsh
parenting may have the positive proximal consequence of reinforcing adaptive behaviours demanded by
the culture of one’s social class, even if at the distal cost of leading lower-income children to grow up less
likely to be open to new people, to explore new areas or to seek out opportunities for autonomy (see
Johnson and Mortimer, 2011).
In reconciling harsh parenting styles with a proposed greater investment in close social relationships,
another possibility is that those living on low incomes would like to be more warm and less strict in their
parenting, but do not have the psychosocial resources available to do so, given the challenges they face
on a daily basis. Given the stress of meeting the growing financial needs of a family in an unstable
economic environment (Haushofer and Fehr, 2014), there may be so many other pressing demands on
one’s mind that the desire to be warm to one’s children recedes into the background. If so, then, as in the
case of thinking processes (see Chapter 4), the pattern of parenting behaviours exhibited more in low-
(than high-) SES families is an understandable case of poorer decision-making; a consequence of a highly
constrained and pressured psychological situation (see Sheehy-Skeffington and Haushofer, 2014).
The above speculations are informed by the evidence reviewed and the conceptual framework proposed
in this report. Given the complexity of the origins and functioning of social values, attributions and
interactions, and the observed consequences of social behaviour throughout the lifespan and across
generations, the relationship between SES and one’s orientation towards the social world is one in urgent
need of empirical illumination.
38
8 Implications for policies and
interventions designed to reduce
poverty
The primary purpose of this evidence review was to provide a comprehensive summary of recent
empirical research on the influence of poverty on key psychological, social and cultural processes relevant
to decision-making. It does so within a broader conceptual framework in which decisions made in
contexts of poverty are understood as potentially functional, in that they may serve proximal goals, if
often at the cost of distal, long-term outcomes. The report was not targeted towards evaluating
behavioural interventions, and thus does not aim to arrive at a definitive set of recommendations for
policy-makers and those working to combat poverty. Nevertheless, there are some general implications
and suggestions that arise from the evidence review, which are laid out in this chapter for further
consideration and assessment.
These implications can be divided into three types: high-level considerations, specific areas for
intervention, and topics where future research is needed.
High-level considerations
The conceptual model proposed at the start of this report arose as a way of summarising emerging
evidence and theory on decision-making in poverty. It then proved a useful lens through which to
understand the findings reviewed, and to consider their consequences for decision-making and life
outcomes. Below is the set of key insights developed from this approach, which are worth carrying
forward to future efforts to understand decision-making in poverty in industrialised countries.
Frame decision-making in poverty as the product of the interaction of individual resources and
powerful socioeconomic and cultural contexts
.
The potent psychosocial impact of the
characteristics of life in poverty, from resource scarcity and environmental instability to low
subjective standing and social exclusion, needs to be explicitly recognised in poverty reduction
efforts. Because much decision-making in poverty is a product of socioeconomic forces shaping
psychological ones, a focus on how decisions are made at the individual or interpersonal level must
not preclude interventions targeting the higher levels (such as the neighbourhood, institutions and
wider societal structures), which are to a large extent responsible for the production of poverty and
inequality in the first place (see Chapter 2; see also Sheehy-Skeffington, 2015).
Consider decision-making in poverty as the product of the workings of general psychosocial
mechanisms. By considering the psychosocial mechanisms that underlie decision-making in a range
of contexts, we can design interventions that apply more widely than those which focus on decision-
making in just one domain (see Chapter 1).
Recognise the cognitive constraints of life in poverty, and their potential impact on decision
quality.
An important step in considering improvements in decisions and behaviours associated with
life in poverty is to recognise how extremely different and challenging such a life is, compared to a
life of wealth. In particular, poverty poses a set of psychological constraints that mean that the
cognitive (and perhaps relational) resources available as a default in middle- or upper-class contexts
are largely absent (see Chapters 4 and 7). Specific interventions (such as those suggested below)
should be designed with this ‘psychological situation of poverty’ (Sheehy-Skeffington and Haushofer,
2014) in mind.
Consider the existence of cognitive skills that are not only constrained in contexts of poverty,
but possibly specialised towards context-specific goals.
Though the psychological constraints of
poverty might trigger worse performance on classic academic and decision-making tasks, they might
trigger better performance on tasks that are tailored to address the needs made salient by poverty
(Sheehy-Skeffington
et al
., 2016 see Chapter 4), or that draw on cognitive skills harnessed by life in
39
high-risk, unpredictable environments (Frankenhuis
et al
., 2016). As research on this possibility is
only now being conducted, policy-makers, and indeed anyone without direct experience of poverty,
should be open to how society might learn effective new decision-making techniques by considering
the adaptive cognitive skill set imbued by an early life of adversity.
Reframe decision-making in poverty as a shift in psychological focus, towards the here, the
now, the actual and those socially close. Whereas the impact of poverty on thinking processes is
often damaging, the influence of poverty on motivation, behaviour and interpersonal processes is
best understood as functional, and as serving important proximal needs (see Chapters 2, 5, 6 and 7).
Focusing only on distal consequences of apparently ‘problem behaviours’ associated with poverty
limits the view of decision-making in such contexts to being suboptimal or irrational. This
representation not only misconstrues the actual functioning of the psychosocial processes involved,
but also denies the very real agency and responsibility possessed by those living at the margins of any
society.
Build into the design of interventions consideration of the proximal functions served by
decisions and behaviours observed in low-income contexts. Using a model that understands
decision-making in poverty in terms of proximal functions and distal consequences (see Chapter 2)
will aid in the design of interventions that are more sustainable than those that focus on distal
consequences alone. A behavioural pattern can be most effectively improved when approached with
an understanding of the reason it is engaged in in the first place, and respect for the decision-maker
as a purposeful and capable agent. One might work with those living in low-income contexts to
replace actions they find detrimental with those they find desirable in the context of distal rewards
and proximal constraints. More generally, before targeting change at the individual level, one might
consider ways to address the constraint that is leading to a proximal focus in the first place (for
examples, see below).
Specific options for intervention
The relationships presented in this report between poverty and the psychosocial mechanisms
underpinning decision-making renders a set of candidate intervention areas worthy of consideration for
addressing poverty in the UK. These are presented below, one level of intervention at a time, and in the
order in which their underpinning insights are introduced in the evidence review.
Individual-level
Design and implement training that attempts to directly improve the functioning of
psychosocial mechanisms. For example:
Intelligence can be increased through high-quality education, widening access to which
should continue to be a government priority (see Chapter 4).
Attention and self-regulation might be improved for all socioeconomic groups through
learning and practising meditation (Tang
et al
., 2007; see Chapters 4 and 5).
Self-efficacy can be developed through training in job skills and financial literacy, which
could be made available to low-income groups via Job Centres (see Chapter 6).
Response efficacy could be increased through information, such as through NHS
communications regarding the influence of health behaviours on life outcomes (see
Chapter 6).
The benefits and feasibility of adaptive coping styles might be conveyed through
communications and support from local primary care centres (see Chapter 6).
Self-esteem and aspirations should be nurtured with sensitivity to the realistic goals,
material needs and sociocultural norms of low-income contexts (see Chapters 6 and 7).
Warm and responsive parenting skills can be modelled and encouraged for all parents
through support groups based at NHS Children’s Centres (see Chapter 7).
Design and implement training in strategies or outlooks that help to mitigate the influence of
poverty on a psychosocial mechanism or its related decision-making processes. For example:
40
Improving parenting styles (see above), as well as improving child interpersonal skills (see
Chapter 7) could buffer the impact of poverty on cognitive functioning (see Chapter 4).
The shift-and-persist mindset can be taught as a way of dealing with obstacles, thus
mitigating their impact on stress and motivation (see Chapter 6).
The link between poverty and low self-worth could be weakened by encouraging those
living in adverse circumstances to think about, and draw on, their existing networks of
social support (see Chapter 6).
Anxiety-reduction techniques such as mindfulness training, if made available at highly
subsidised rates through charities, could equip people to help reduce the psychological
pressure induced by living in or near poverty (see Chapter 6), with knock-on effects on
self-regulation (see Chapter 5).
Encouraging low-income or unemployed job applicants to think about intrinsic job
characteristics, such as autonomy and flexibility, could increase their chances of obtaining
work that supports self-efficacy and response efficacy (see Chapter 6).
The social segregation of those living in low-SES contexts could be reduced by teaching
at secondary school the benefits of social networks to career progression, decreasing the
likelihood that only those from higher-SES backgrounds will seek to make new
connections at college and work (see Chapter 7).
Diversity in cultural values could be taught as part of secondary school civics education
classes. This could prepare students from working class backgrounds or those in poverty
to succeed in contexts where self-advancement values predominate, while also educating
middle- and high-income children about the importance of self-transcendent values (see
Chapter 7).
Fixed beliefs about the limited academic or career potential of those low in SES should be
challenged as part of career advice in secondary school, to be replaced with beliefs
regarding the malleability of intelligence and the mobility of social position (cf. Dweck,
2000; Oyserman
et al
., 2011; see Chapter 7).
Neighbourhood-level
Develop neighbourhood policies and initiatives that can improve the psychosocial processes of
all inhabitants.
For example:
Strengthen social routes to self-efficacy, such as through group-based decision-making
structures in residential complexes (Fritsche
et al
., 2013; see Chapter 6).
Reduce the sense of social exclusion (or, conversely, superiority) felt by those at the
bottom (or the top, respectively) of a community’s socioeconomic hierarchy by
combatting income-based segregation and ensuring that low-, middle- and high-income
communities have opportunities to interact on a daily basis, such as through mixed
residential developments, neighbourhood spaces and public transport (see Chapter 7).
Tackle the propagation of aggressive norms in low-income neighbourhoods through
enhancing the sense of shared responsibility of the community for the behaviour of its
members (Sampson, 2006; see Chapters 5 and 7).
Develop neighbourhood policies and initiatives that can mitigate the influence of poverty on a
psychosocial mechanism or its related decision-making processes.
For example:
Activate the benefits of increased social support for self-appraisal and self-efficacy by
creating spaces in low-income communities for residents to congregate (see Chapter 6).
Activate the benefits of spiritual support for self-appraisal by supporting faith-based
places and groups in low-income communities that cater to a diversity of inhabitants (see
Chapter 6).
41
Create opportunities for the formation of tightly knit friendship groups not defined in
terms of societal deviance, through local youth clubs and extracurricular school activities
(Chapter 7).
Institutional
Design and reform institutions in order to ensure they work in favour of, rather than against,
the psychosocial processes of their users.
For example:
Strengthen self-efficacy and response efficacy of all employees by providing
opportunities for ongoing skills training and autonomy in job design and work schedule,
respectively (see Chapter 5).
Enhance academic motivation and sense of belonging by investing in school facilities in
low-income areas. Not only the material resources, but also the appearance and cultural
norms of a school, matter for student motivation and cohesion (see Chapters 5 and 7).
Improve perceived inclusion and cultural fit by making institutions more welcoming to
working-class users who are likely to have an interdependent sense of self.
Reconsider
the implicit cultural norms and values of schools, universities and workplaces, which may
privilege the independent forms of agency and interpersonal style that originate almost
exclusively from middle- and upper-class contexts (Stephens
et al
., 2012; see Chapter 7).
Ensure institutions help in mitigating, as opposed to exacerbating, the negative influence of
poverty on psychosocial mechanisms and decision-making. For example:
Mitigate the psychosocial disruption of life on a low income by providing low-earning
workers a sense of job security and long-term commitment, thus reducing uncertainty
and stress (see Chapters 2 and 6).
Prevent constraints on executive functioning from acting as a barrier to low-income
groups’ engagement with financial and public institutions, by making communications and
application forms from such institutions more clear and succinct (Mullainathan and Shafir,
2013; see Chapter 4).
Reduce the impact on occupational performance of distraction and strain caused by
family-related financial troubles or parenting demands, by ensuring workplaces provide
adequate support for challenges faced outside of work, such as through subsidised
childcare and parental leave (see Chapter 4).
Enhance the approach orientation and performance of all students or employees by
designing school or workplace assessments to focus less on competition, and more on
mastery and collaboration.
As academic or occupational contexts focused on relative
performance can enhance perceived difficulty for low-SES students and employees of
competing with others from higher social class backgrounds, such contexts should be
replaced by ones that focus on the merits of learning and task completion, or which draw
on collaborative norms more intuitive to those growing up in interdependent social class
contexts (see Chapters 4, 5 and 7).
Reduce perceived exclusion (see Chapter 7) and performance disparities (see Chapter 4)
by tackling prejudice and discrimination based on social class, whether in the form of
micro-aggressions, ostracism or bullying, or in the use of inferences drawn from
interpersonal mannerisms, dress or accent to inform hiring and promotion decisions.
Sensitivity to the ongoing reality of socioeconomic disadvantage and class-based
prejudice should be at the centre of institutional diversity and anti-discrimination efforts.
Such efforts could include the celebration of socioeconomic diversity, and the mandatory
training of all of those involved in promotion and hiring decisions in the pervasiveness of
social class stereotypes.
42
Societal
Reduce the psychological burden of poverty, by supporting, strengthening and widening the
government-provided safety net for those living in or near poverty, or experiencing temporary
financial hardship. As life in poverty brings with it so many characteristics that have a potent
psychosocial impact (see Chapters 4, 5, 6 and 7), decision-making in poverty can only be improved
when such material strains are at least partially relieved.
Minimise the psychological impact of relative socioeconomic standing.
The importance of
subjective social standing for cognition (see Chapter 4), motivation (see Chapter 5), self-belief (see
Chapter 6) and perceived inclusion (see Chapter 7) implies that societal conditions which increase its
salience will have myriad negative psychosocial consequences for those at the bottom. The UK
Government should build such psychosocial considerations into its fiscal and regulatory decisions as
it considers responses to rising economic inequality.
Improve public discourse on the normative dimension of socioeconomic status (SES).
To the
extent that the link between SES and self-esteem is culturally-specific (see Gnambs
et al
., 2015), and
the valuing of self-enhancement is class-specific (see Rea, 2016) it might be worth questioning,
through media communications and public debate, the ways in which the UK middle class cultural
context links sense of personal worth to material resources (see Chapters 6 and 7).
Challenge public representations of poverty and decision-making. Stereotypes about ‘suboptimal’
or ‘wilfully irrational’ behaviours associated with low-income groups are rendered problematic by the
findings of this report, as they neglect the myriad psychosocial constraints built into the experience
of life near or below the poverty line, and do little justice to the purposive and adaptive nature of
much decision-making in the face of adversity (see Chapter 2). So that those in the lowest earning
groups in society are truly empowered by behaviourally focused anti-poverty interventions, public
engagement and education should do justice to the complexity and contextual rationality of
decision-making in contexts of poverty.
Areas in need of research
Though the model and evidence presented above holds the potential to radically shift our understanding
of poverty and decision-making, it will need to achieve robust empirical support for all of its components
in order to be compelling in policy and public circles. This set of systematic reviews pointed to a pressing
need to conduct rigorous research on the following topics, in order to strengthen and deepen our
understanding of decision-making in contexts of poverty.
The proposed pathways linking poverty and decision-making, specifically:
the relationship between poverty and psychological distance (on temporal, existential,
social and spatial dimensions), in order to test whether there is indeed a pattern of
constriction of focus to the proximal realm, and thus to guide stronger behaviour change
interventions (see Chapter 2); and
the relationship between poverty and construal level (abstract versus concrete
information-processing), in order to understand the underlying mechanism explaining the
proximal focus in decision-making associated with low SES; understanding this
relationship will also reveal what kinds of cognitive task might yield enhanced, versus
impaired, performance by those low in SES (see Chapters 2 and 4).
The potential for adaptiveness and specialisation in cognitive processes as a result of life in poverty
(see Chapter 2). Aside from its intrinsic scientific interest, understanding this might, firstly, enable the
design of school and occupational assessment tasks that do justice to the cognitive abilities of those
growing up in low-SES families. Secondly, it opens up the possibility that specific decision-making
skills could be learned from those growing up in adverse circumstances that might help all people
deal with situations of immediate need or threat.
The relationship between poverty and specific understudied psychosocial mechanisms, namely:
43
susceptibility to heuristics and biases, where there is a paucity of research looking at links
with SES (see Chapter 4);
intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation, where there are conflicting findings regarding links
with SES (see Chapter 5);
risk-taking in specific contexts, where there is need for both empirical (what is the link
between SES and risk-taking?) and normative (when is risk-taking desirable?) clarification
(see Chapter 5);
sociocultural norms, where research needs to be conducted in specific contexts relevant
to urban and rural poverty in the UK (see Chapter 5);
health-related self-efficacy, where the more recent research seems not to replicate
previously observed links with SES (see Chapter 6);
interpersonal perceptions and behaviours, where group membership remains to be
studied as a boundary condition to observe relationships with SES (see Chapter 7); and
traditional values, where more evidence is needed to be sure of the robustness of
observed links with SES (see Chapter 7).
44
9 Conclusion
The study of decision-making in contexts of poverty is fraught with difficulty, for both normative and
empirical reasons. Normatively, a focus on the individual actions and behaviours of those living in or near
poverty risks occluding the role of wider social structural factors in exacerbating and sustaining poverty
even while economic growth at the national level continues. Empirically, there is such a wide set of
decisions that matter for life outcomes, and such multifaceted ways in which they may be affected by
and, in turn, affect poverty, that a comprehensive understanding might be unfeasible to achieve, and
impossible to communicate easily.
This overarching evidence review has attempted to tackle these challenges by reporting on social science
evidence, and by pushing forward social science theory, pertaining to the question at hand. At the
empirical level, by focusing on core psychosocial processes that underpin decision-making in a range of
contexts, the project aimed to shed light on the mediating mechanisms of poverty’s influence on
behaviour. Towards this end, it presented results from multiple systematic reviews of the evidence for the
relationship between socioeconomic status (SES) and 15 key psychosocial mechanisms. This revealed
findings that both corroborate and challenge previously established knowledge, while also identifying
pressing gaps to be filled by future empirical work. Caveats must be placed on the depth and
generalisability of the findings, given that much of the evidence was in the form of correlations obtained
from cross-sectional samples, the vast majority of which were located outside of the UK. Nevertheless,
they point to the potential for interventions that might improve psychosocial functioning, while revealing
shortcomings in existing approaches that have not previously been considered.
At the conceptual level, the authors have introduced models from social psychology to help organise and
understand patterns of decision-making in poverty, arguing for its reframing in terms of distal
consequences and proximal functions. This reveals the extent to which people living in or near poverty
behave in rational and purposive ways in response to challenging environmental constraints. It thus allows
one to replace the normatively problematic approach of ‘blaming the poor’ for decisions perceived to be
unwise with one that looks squarely at the role of contextual forces in shaping such decisions in
understandable ways. The goal is to have provided inspiration and guidance for the design of substantive
and sustainable interventions at multiple levels, from the individual up to the societal.
In summary, the authors hope to have demonstrated that rigorous empirical research, when combined
with theorising that recognises the power of context and of people’s agency in navigating it, can lay the
ground for understandings of decision-making in poverty that are faithful less to assumptions and
stereotypes, than to the actual experiences of those whose lives it touches.
45
Glossary
Affect/affective – concerning emotions or feelings
Approach orientation – a person’s behavioural orientation towards their goals; the extent to which one
is motivated to act in order to achieve one’s goals, and focused on the rewards on offer if one
accomplishes them
Arousal – raised activation or readiness of a psychological or physiological mechanism
Attribution – an inference made from a person’s observed behaviour about their internal traits or
intentions
Boundary conditions – limits put on the set of circumstances under which an observed relationship is
expected to occur
Cognition – psychological processes involving thought and information-processing
Construal level – the level of abstraction (versus concreteness) with which one processes information at
any given time
Coping style – one’s general pattern of response to stress; this can include accepting or confronting the
stressor, attempting to adapt oneself or the environment to reduce stress, or ignoring/denying the
source of stress
Cross-sectional – involving the study of (usually, correlations between) processes and outcomes in one
group of people measured at one time-point
Decision-making - the thought process of selecting a choice or action from a set of available options
Executive functions/executive functioning – a set of basic cognitive processes that monitor and
control thought and action
Experimental – involving the random assignment of study participants to be exposed to one or other
kind of treatment; any subsequent differences in outcome variables between conditions can be assumed
to be caused by the experimental treatment
Externalising behaviour – a problematic behavioural pattern involving negative actions towards others,
such as disobeying rules, physical aggression or intimidation
Extrinsic motivation – motivation to perform a task because of instrumental benefits that result from it
Future (also temporal) discounting – the extent to which one attributes greater value to a reward the
sooner it is received
Heuristics – simple decision rules used for fast decision-making
Ingroup bias – the extent to which one has more positive social evaluations of members of one’s own
social group than of non-members
Inhibition orientation – the extent to which one has diminished motivation to act in order to achieve
one’s goals, and/or is focused on threats and barriers standing in the way of them
Inhibitory (also cognitive or executive) control – one of the executive functions, the ability to hold
back a tempting (‘facilitated’ or ‘prepotent’) but incorrect response
46
Internal (versus external) locus of control – the extent to which one sees events as under the control
of the self (versus environmental factors such as chance or other people’s behaviour)
Internalising behaviour – a problematic behavioural pattern involving social withdrawal and negative
perceptions of the self, sometimes accompanied by self-punishment of some kind
Interpersonal warmth – the amount of positive affect and co-operative intent conveyed in one’s
interactions with others
Intrinsic motivation – motivation to perform a task because of rewards one gains out of the
performance of the task itself
Longitudinal – involving the study of processes and outcomes in the same group of people measured at
more than one time-point, in which associations are assessed between initial levels of a predictor variable
and later levels of an outcome variable
Machiavellianism – a personality trait centred on adherence to a world view in which everyone is self-
serving, and thus exploitation and manipulation of others for personal ends can be justified
Mediator/mediate – (that which) acts as a pathway, or intermediate mechanism, through which a causal
variable influences an outcome variable
Psychological distance – the proximal (versus distal) psychological location of a mental process or
representation with reference to the self; consists of temporal (now versus later), existential (actual
versus hypothetical), social (socially close versus socially distant) and spatial (here versus far away)
dimensions
Psychological situation (of poverty) – the parameters and cues regarding current challenges that are
triggered by the experience of life in poverty (e.g. instability, resource scarcity, low social status) and thus
make some behavioural responses more facilitated than others
Risk aversion – the tendency to prefer certain rewards over (often larger) rewards of uncertain
probability
Risk proneness – the tendency to prefer rewards of uncertain probability over (often smaller) rewards of
certain probability
Selective attention – one of the executive functions, the ability to focus on a target stimulus despite the
presence of distractor stimuli
Self-concept – a person’s image of herself or himself
Self-efficacy – the extent to which one feels able to learn new skills and to perform the actions required
by a task
Self-esteem – how a person evaluates herself or himself in relation to others
Self-regulation – the extent to which one acts in line with long-term over short-term goals, involving
impulse control and delay of gratification
Social perception – representations of how others in one’s environment tend to behave, as well as one’s
style of attribution of the traits and intentions of others, based on their behaviours
Sociocultural norms – behavioural patterns and expectations originating from members of one’s
surrounding family, community, colleagues or peers
Subjective social status personal perception of where one sits on the socioeconomic ladder of one’s
community or society
47
Response efficacy – the extent to which one feels the actions one performs will have the desired
impact, or will matter for one’s life outcomes
Values – desired goals or outcomes, guiding principles
48
References
Aarøe, L. and Petersen, M. B. (2013) ‘Hunger games fluctuations in blood glucose levels influence support for social welfare’,
Psychological Science
, 24(12), pp. 2550–56
Abraham, C., Sheeran, P. and Henderson, M. (2011). 'Extending social cognition models of health behaviour',
Health Education
Research
, 26(4), pp. 624–37
Adler, N. E. and Rehkopf, D. H. (2008) 'U.S. disparities in health: descriptions, causes, and mechanisms',
Annual Review of Public
Health
, 29, pp. 235–52
Aguayo, D., Herman, K., Ojeda, L. and Flores, L. Y. (2011) 'Culture predicts Mexican Americans' college self-efficacy and college
performance',
Journal of Diversity in Higher Education
, 4(2), pp. 79–89
Alivernini, F. and Lucidi, F. (2011) 'Relationship between social context, self-efficacy, motivation, academic achievement, and
intention to drop out of high school: a longitudinal study',
Journal of Educational Research
, 104(4), pp. 241–52
Ambrasat, J., Von Scheve, C., Conrad, M., Schauenburg, G. and Schrod̈er, T. (2014) 'Consensus and stratification in the affective
meaning of human sociality',
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America
, 111(22), pp.
8001–6
Ardila, A., Roselli, M., Matute, E. and Guajardo, S. (2005) 'The influence of the parents’ educational level on the development of
executive functions',
Developmental Neuropsychology
, 28, pp. 539–60
Arslan, A. (2013) 'Investigation of relationship between sources of self-efficacy beliefs of secondary school students and some
variables',
Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice
, 13(4), pp. 1983–93
Auger, N., Lo, E., Cantinotti, M. and O'Loughlin, J. (2010) 'Impulsivity and socio-economic status interact to increase the risk of
gambling onset among youth',
Addiction
, 105(12), pp. 2176–83
Bandura, A. (2001) 'Social cognitive theory: an agentic perspective',
Annual review of psychology
, 52(1), pp. 1–26
Bardi, A., Calogero, R. M. and Mullen, B. (2008) 'A new archival approach to the study of values and value--behavior relations:
validation of the value lexicon',
Journal of Applied Psychology
, 93(3), p. 483
Bardi, A. and Goodwin, R. (2011) 'The dual route to value change: Individual processes and cultural moderators',
Journal of cross-
cultural psychology
, 42(2), pp. 271–87
Bardi, A. and Schwartz, S. H. (2003) 'Values and behavior: strength and structure of relations',
Personality and social psychology
bulletin
, 29(10), pp. 1207–20
Barefoot, J. C., Peterson, B. L., Dahlstrom, W. G., Siegler, I. C., Anderson, N. B. and Williams, R. B., Jr. (1991) 'Hostility patterns and
health implications: correlates of Cook-Medley hostility scale scores in a national survey',
Health Psychology
, 10, pp. 18–24
Bassett, H. H., Denham, S., Wyatt, T. M. and Warren-Khot, H. K. (2012) 'Refining the preschool self-regulation assessment for use in
preschool classrooms',
Infant and Child Development
, 21(6), pp. 596–616
Baydar, N. and Akcinar, B. (2015) 'Ramifications of socioeconomic differences for three year old children and their families in
Turkey',
Early Childhood Research Quarterly
, 33, pp. 33–48
Beal, S. J. and Crockett, L. J. (2013) 'Adolescents' occupational and educational goals: a test of reciprocal relations',
Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology
, 34(5), pp. 219–29
Beceren, B. Ö. (2010)
Determining multiple intelligences pre-school children (4-6 age) in learning process.
Paper presented at the
2nd World Conference on Educational Sciences, WCES-2010, Istanbul, Turkey
Behavioural Insights Team (2010)
MINDSPACE: Influencing behaviour through public policy
. London: Cabinet Office/Institute for
Government. Available from: http://38r8om2xjhhl25mw24492dir.wpengine.netdna-cdn.com/wp-
content/uploads/2015/07/MINDSPACE.pdf [accessed 11 October 2016]
Berger, N. and Archer, J. (2015) 'School socio-economic status and student socio-academic achievement goals in upper secondary
contexts',
Social Psychology of Education
, 19(1), pp. 175–94
Bernier, A., Beauchamp, M. H., Carlson, S. M. and Lalonde, G. (2015) 'A secure base from which to regulate: attachment security in
toddlerhood as a predictor of executive functioning at school entry',
Developmental Psychology
, 51(9), pp. 1177–89
49
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Bordeleau, S. and Carrier, J. (2010a) 'Relations between physiological and cognitive regulatory systems:
infant sleep regulation and subsequent executive functioning',
Child Development
, 81(6), pp. 1739–52
Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M. and Whipple, N. (2010b) ‘From external regulation to selfregulation: early parenting precursors of young
children’s executive functioning’,
Child Development
, 81(1), pp. 326–39
Bertrand, M., Mullainathan, S. and Shafir, E. (2006) 'Behavioural economics and marketing in aid of decision making among the poor',
Journal of Public Policy and Marketing
, 25, pp. 8–23
Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L. and Hurt, H. (2015) 'Socioeconomic (SES) differences in language are evident in female infants at 7
months of age',
Early Human Development
, 91(12), pp. 719–24
Bethell, C. D., Newacheck, P., Hawes, E. and Halfon, N. (2014) 'Changing epidemiology of children's health. Adverse childhood
experiences: assessing the impact on health and school engagement and the mitigating role of resilience',
Health Affairs
,
33(12), pp. 2106–15
Billings, A. G. and Moos, R. H. (1981) 'The role of coping responses and social resources in attenuating the stress of life
events',
Journal of behavioral medicine
, 4(2), pp. 139–57
Bodovski, K. (2014) 'Adolescents’ emerging habitus: the role of early parental expectations and practices',
British Journal of
Sociology of Education
, 35(3), pp. 389–412
Bodovski, K. and Youn, M.-J. (2011) 'The long term effects of early acquired skills and behaviors on young children's achievement in
Literacy and Mathematics',
Journal of Early Childhood Research
, 9(1), pp. 4–19
Boelema, S. R., Harakeh, Z., Ormel, J., Hartman, C. A., Vollebergh, W. A. M. and van Zandvoort, M. J. E. (2014) 'Executive functioning
shows differential maturation from early to late adolescence: longitudinal findings from a TRAILS study',
Neuropsychology
,
28(2), pp. 177–87
Booij, A. S., van Praag, B. M. S. and van de Kuilen, G. (2009)
A parametric analysis of prospect theory's functionals for the general
population.
IZA Discussion Papers, No. 4117. Bonn, Germany: IZA
Bornstein, M. H. and Putnick, D. L. (2012) ‘Stability of language in childhood: a multiage, multidomain, multimeasure, and
multisource study’,
Developmental Psychology
, 48(2), pp. 477–91
Bowden, M. P. and Doughney, J. (2010) ‘Socio-economic status, cultural diversity and the aspirations of secondary students in the
western suburbs of Melbourne, Australia’,
Higher Education: The International Journal of Higher Education and Educational
Planning
, 59(1), pp. 115–29
Boxer, P., Goldstein, S. E., DeLorenzo, T., Savoy, S. and Mercado, I. (2011) ‘Educational aspiration-expectation discrepancies: relation
to socioeconomic and academic risk-related factors’,
Journal of Adolescence
, 34(4), pp. 609–17
Brandt, M. J., Wetherell, G. and Henry, P. J. (2014) ‘Changes in income predict change in social trust: a longitudinal analysis’,
Political
Psychology
, 36(6), pp. 761–68
Bronte-Tinkew, J., Horowitz, A. and Carrano, J. (2010) ‘Aggravation and stress in parenting: associations with coparenting and
father engagement among resident fathers’,
Journal of Family Issues
, 31(4), pp. 525–55
Browne, D. T., O'Connor, T. G., Meunier, J. C. and Jenkins, J. M. (2012) ‘The role of parental personality traits in differential
parenting’,
Journal of Family Psychology
, 26(4), pp. 542–53
Bullock, H. E., Fraser Wyche, K. and Williams, W. R. (2001) ‘Media images of the poor’,
Journal of Social Issues
, 57(2), pp. 229–46
Bullock, H. E., Williams, W. R. and Limbert, W. M. (2003) ‘Predicting support for welfare policies: the impact of attributions and
beliefs about inequality’,
Journal of Poverty
, 7(3), pp. 35–56
Businelle, M. S., Kendzor, D. E., Reitzel, L. R., Vidrine, J. I., Castro, Y., Mullen, P. D., Velasquez, M. M., Cofta-Woerpel, L., Cinciripini, P.
M., Greisinger, A. J. and Wetter, D. W. (2013) ‘Pathways linking socioeconomic status and postpartum smoking relapse’,
Annals of Behavioral Medicine
, 45(2), pp. 180–91
Callan, M. J., Shead, N. W. and Olson, J. M. (2011) ‘Personal relative deprivation, delay discounting, and gambling’,
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology
, 101(5), pp. 955–73
Campbell Collaboration.(2001)
Guidelines for preparation of review protocols v1.0
. Available from:
https://www.campbellcollaboration.org/images/pdf/plain-language/C2_Protocols_guidelines_v1.pdf [accessed 7 November
2016]
Caner, A. and Okten, C. (2010) ‘Risk and career choice: evidence from Turkey’,
Economics of Education Review
, 29(6), pp. 1060–
75
50
Capozza, D., Andrighetto, L., Di Bernardo, G. A. and Falvo, R. (2012) ‘Does status affect intergroup perceptions of humanity?’,
Group
Processes and Intergroup Relations
, 15(4), pp. 363–77
Carr, H. L. and Vignoles, V. L. (2011) ‘Keeping up with the Joneses: status projection as symbolic selfcompletion’,
European Journal
of Social Psychology
, 41(4), pp. 518–27
Carvalho, L. S., Meier, S. and Wang, S. W. (2016) ‘Poverty and economic decision-making: evidence from changes in financial
resources at payday’,
American Economic Review
, 106(2), pp. 260–84
Cattell, V. (2001) ‘Poor people, poor places, and poor health: the mediating role of social networks and social capital’,
Social science
and medicine
, 52(10), pp. 1501–16
Chapman, B. P., Fiscella, K., Kawachi, I. and Duberstein, P. R. (2010) ‘Personality, socioeconomic status, and all-cause mortality in the
United States’,
American Journal of Epidemiology,
171(1), pp. 83–92
Chen, E., Lee, W. K., Cavey, L. and Ho, A. (2013) ‘Role models and the psychological characteristics that buffer lowsocioeconomic
status youth from cardiovascular risk’,
Child Development,
84(4), pp. 1241–52
Chen, E. and Miller, G. E. (2012) ‘“Shift-and-persist” strategies: why low socioeconomic status isn’t always bad for
health’,
Perspectives on Psychological Science
, 7(2), pp. 135–58
Chen, E., Strunk, R. C., Trethewey, A., Schreier, H. M. C., Maharaj, N. and Miller, G. E. (2011) ‘Resilience in low-socioeconomic-status
children with asthma: adaptations to stress’,
The Journal Of Allergy And Clinical Immunology
, 128(5), pp. 970–76
Chen, Y.-J. and Tang, T. L.-P. (2013) ‘The bright and dark sides of religiosity among university students: do gender, college major,
and income matter?’,
Journal of Business Ethics,
115(3), pp. 531–53
Chow, H. P. H. (2011) ‘Procrastination among undergraduate students: effects of emotional intelligence, school life, self-evaluation,
and self- efficacy’,
Alberta Journal of Educational Research
, 57(2), pp. 234–40
Chowdry, H., Crawford, C. and Goodman, A. (2011) ‘The role of attitudes and behaviours in explaining socio-economic differences
in attainment at age 16’,
Longitudinal and Life Course Studies,
2(1), pp. 59–76
Cialdini, R. B. and Trost, M. R. (1998) ‘Social influence: social norms, conformity and compliance’. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, L.
Gardner (eds),
The Handbook of Social Psychology, Vols 1 and 3
(4
th
ed.), pp. 151–92. New York: McGraw-Hill
Clery, E., Lee, L. and Kunz, S. (2013)
Public attitudes to poverty and welfare, 1983–2011: analysis using British Social Attitudes data.
York: JRF
Cochran, D. B., Wang, E. W., Stevenson, S. J., Johnson, L. E. and Crews, C. (2011).A’dolescent occupational aspirations: test of
Gottfredson's theory of circumscription and compromise’,
Career Development Quarterly
, 59(5), pp. 412–27
Colasante, T., Zuffianò, A. and Malti, T. (2015) ‘Do moral emotions buffer the anger-aggression link in children and adolescents?’,
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
, 41, pp. 1–7
Condon, M. and Holleque, M. (2013) ‘Entering politics: general selfefficacy and voting behavior among young people’,
Political
Psychology
, 34(2), pp. 167–81
Conger, R. D., Martin, M. J., Masarik, A. S., Widaman, K. F. and Donnellan, M. B. (2015) ‘Social and economic antecedents and
consequences of adolescent aggressive personality: predictions from the interactionist model’,
Development and
Psychopathology
, 27(4, Pt 1), pp. 1111–27
Conner, M., McEachan, R., Jackson, C., McMillan, B., Woolridge, M. and Lawton, R. (2013) ‘Moderating effect of socioeconomic
status on the relationship between health cognitions and behaviors’,
Annals of Behavioral Medicine
, 46(1), pp. 19–30
Cremers, H. P., Oenema, A., Mercken, L., Candel, M. and De Vries, H. (2014) ‘Explaining socio-economic differences in intention to
smoke among primary school children’,
BMC Public Health
, 14(1), Article 191
Dahlbäck, O. (1990) ‘Personality and risk-taking’,
Personality and individual differences
, 11(12), pp. 1235–42
Dalton, P. S., Ghosal, S. and Mani, A. (2016) ‘Poverty and aspirations failure’,
The Economic Journal
, 126(590), pp. 165–88
D’Angiulli, A., Van Roon, P. M., Weinberg, J., Oberlander, T. F., Grunau, R. E., Hertzman, C. and Maggi, S. (2012) ‘Frontal EEG/ERP
correlates of attentional processes, cortisol and motivational states in adolescents from lower and higher socioeconomic
status’,
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience
, 6, Article 306
Damian, R. I. and Roberts, B. W. (2015) ‘The associations of birth order with personality and intelligence in a representative sample
of U.S. high school students’,
Journal of Research in Personality
, 58, pp. 96–105
51
Danvers, A. F., Scott, B. G., Sandler, I. N., Ayers, T. S., Wolchik, S. A., Luecken, L. J., . . . Shiota, M. N. (2015) ‘Effect of economic
distress on physical health is mediated by low coping efficacy and cardiac reactivity to negative stimuli’,
Psychophysiology
, 52,
pp. S28–S28
Davies, P. and Boruch, R. (2001) ‘The Campbell Collaboration’,
British Medical Journal
, 323, pp. 294–5
Day, C. A. and Burns, B. M. (2011) ‘Characterizing the achievement motivation orientation of children from low- and middle-income
families’,
Early Education and Development
, 22(1), pp. 105–27
Day, S., Papataxiarchis, E. and Stewart, M. (1999) ‘Consider the lilies of the field’. In S. Day, E. Papataxiarchis and M. Stewart (eds),
Lilies of the field: marginal people who live for the moment
,
pp. 1–24. Boulder, CO: Westview Press
de Bruin, W.B., Parker, A.M. and Fischhoff, B. (2007) ‘Individual differences in adult decision-making competence’,
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology
, 92, pp. 938–56
DeFlorio, L. and Beliakoff, A. (2015) ‘Socioeconomic status and preschoolers' mathematical knowledge: the contribution of home
activities and parent beliefs’,
Early Education and Development
, 26(3), pp. 319–41
Denham, S. A., Kalb, S., Way, E., Warren-Khot, H., Rhoades, B. L. and Bassett, H. H. (2013) ‘Social and emotional information
processing in preschoolers: indicator of early school success?’,
Early Child Development and Care
, 183(5), pp. 667–88
Department for Work and Pensions (2015)
2010 to 2015 government policy: welfare reform
. Available from:
https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/2010-to-2015-government-policy-welfare-reform [accessed 7 November
2016]
Diamond, J. B. and Huguley, J. P. (2014) ‘Testing the oppositional culture explanation in desegregated schools: the impact of racial
differences in academic orientations on school performance’,
Social Forces
, 93(2), pp. 747–77
Dolan, P. and Galizzi, M. M. (2015) ‘Like ripples on a pond: behavioral spillovers and their implications for research and
policy’,
Journal of Economic Psychology
, 47, pp. 1–16
Dweck, C. S. (2000)
Self-theories: their role in motivation, personality, and development
. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press
Dweck C. S. and Leggett E. L. (1988) ‘A social-cognitive approach to motivation and personality’,
Psychological Review,
95, pp.
256–73
Dzewaltowski, D. A., Geller, K. S., Rosenkranz, R. R. and Karteroliotis, K. (2010) ‘Children's self-efficacy and proxy efficacy for after-
school physical activity’,
Psychology of Sport and Exercise
, 11(2), pp. 100–06
Eckel, C. C., Grossman, P. J., Johnson, C. A., de Oliveira, A. C. M., Rojas, C. and Wilson, R. K. (2012) ‘School environment and risk
preferences: experimental evidence’,
Journal of Risk and Uncertainty
, 45(3), pp. 265–92
Eisenberg, N., Vidmar, M., Spinrad, T. L., Eggum, N. D., Edwards, A., Gaertner, B. and Kupfer, A. (2010) ‘Mothers' teaching strategies
and children's effortful control: a longitudinal study’,
Developmental Psychology
, 46(5), pp. 1294–1308
Ellingsen, R., Baker, B. L., Blacher, J. and Crnic, K. (2014) ‘Resilient parenting of children at developmental risk across middle
childhood’,
Research in Developmental Disabilities
, 35(6), pp. 1364–74
Ellis, B. J., Del Giudice, M., Dishion, T. J., Figueredo, A. J., Gray, P., Griskevicius, V., Hawley, P.H., Jacobs, W. J., James, J., Volk, A. A.
and Wilson, D. S. (2012) ‘The evolutionary basis of risky adolescent behavior: Implications for science, policy, and practice’,
Developmental Psychology
, 48, pp. 598–623
Endler, N. S. and Parker, J. D. (1990) ‘Multidimensional assessment of coping: a critical evaluation’,
Journal of personality and social
psychology
, 58(5), p. 844
Engle R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E., Conway, A. R. A. (1999) ‘Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid
intelligence: a latent-variable approach’,
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General
, 128, pp. 309–31
Erzinger, A. B. and Steiger, A. E. (2014) ‘Intergenerational transmission of maternal and paternal parenting beliefs: the moderating
role of interaction quality’,
European Journal of Developmental Psychology
, 11(2), pp. 177–95
Evans, G. W., Fuller-Rowell, T. E. and Doan, S. N. (2012) ‘Childhood cumulative risk and obesity: the mediating role of self-
regulatory ability’,
Pediatrics
, 129(1), pp. e68–73
Fahlman, M. M., McCaughtry, N., Martin, J. and Shen, B. (2010) ‘Racial and socioeconomic disparities in nutrition behaviors: targeted
interventions needed’,
Journal of Nutrition Education and Behavior
, 42(1), pp. 10–16
Falci, C. D. (2011) ‘Self-esteem and mastery trajectories in high school by social class and gender’,
Social Science Research
, 40(2),
pp. 586–601
52
Fatas, E., Jimenez, F. and Morales, A. J. (2011) ‘Controlling for initial endowment and experience in binary choice tasks’,
Journal of
Risk and Uncertainty
, 43(3), pp. 227–43
Fell, F. and Hewstone, M. (2015)
Psychological perspectives on poverty.
York: JRF
Flensborg-Madsen, T. and Mortensen, E. L. (2014) ‘Infant SES as a predictor of personality–is the association mediated by
intelligence?’,
Plos One
, 9(7), e103846
Flouri, E., Midouhas, E. and Joshi, H. (2014) ‘Family poverty and trajectories of children's emotional and behavioural problems: the
moderating roles of self-regulation and verbal cognitive ability’,
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology
, 42(6), pp. 1043–56
Flouri, E., Midouhas, E. and Joshi, H. (2015a) ‘Family and neighbourhood risk and children's problem behaviour: the moderating role
of intelligence’,
Intelligence
, 53, pp. 33–42
Flouri, E., Tsivrikos, D., Akhtar, R. and Midouhas, E. (2015b) ‘Neighbourhood, school and family determinants of children's aspirations
in primary school’,
Journal of Vocational Behavior
, 87, pp. 71–9
Frankenhuis, W. E. and de Weerth, C. (2013) ‘Does early-life exposure to stress shape or impair cognition?’,
Current Directions in
Psychological Science
, 22(5), pp. 407–12
Frankenhuis, W. E., Panchanathan, K. and Nettle, D. (2016) ‘Cognition in harsh and unpredictable environments’,
Current Opinion in
Psychology
, 7, pp. 76–80
Freeney, Y. and O'Connell, M. (2010) ‘Wait for it: delay-discounting and academic performance among an Irish adolescent sample’,
Learning and Individual Differences
, 20(3), pp. 231–6
Fritsche, I., Jonas, E., Ablasser, C., Beyer, M., Kuban, J., Manger, A. M. and Schultz, M. (2013) ‘The power of we: evidence for group-
based control’,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
, 49(1), pp. 19–32
Frostick, C., Phillips, G., Renton, A. and Moore, D. (2015) ‘The educational and employment aspirations of adolescents from areas of
high deprivation in London’,
Journal of Youth and Adolescence
, 45(6), pp. 1126–40
Fujita, K. (2011) ‘On conceptualizing self-control as more than the effortful inhibition of impulses’,
Personality and Social
Psychology Review
, 15(4), pp. 352–66
Garriott, P. O., Flores, L. Y. and Martens, M. P. (2013) ‘Predicting the math/science career goals of low-income prospective first-
generation college students’,
Journal of Counseling Psychology,
60(2), pp. 200–09
Gilens, M. (1999)
Why Americans hate welfare: race, media, and the politics of antipoverty policy
. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago
Press
Glasscock, D. J., Andersen, J. H., Labriola, M., Rasmussen, K. and Hansen, C. D. (2013) ‘Can negative life events and coping style help
explain socioeconomic differences in perceived stress among adolescents? A cross-sectional study based on the West Jutland
cohort study’,
BMC Public Health
, 13, Article 532
Gnambs, T., Stiglbauer, B. and Selenko, E. (2015) ‘Psychological effects of (non)employment: a cross-national comparison of the
United States and Japan’,
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology,
56(6), pp. 659–69
Goodman, E., Adler, N. E., Kawachi, I., Frazier, A. L., Huang, B. and Colditz, G. A. (2001) ‘Adolescents’ perceptions of social status:
development and evaluation of a new indicator’,
Pediatrics
, 108,
pp.
1–8
Gore, J., Holmes, K., Smith, M., Southgate, E. and Albright, J. (2015) ‘Socioeconomic status and the career aspirations of Australian
school students: testing enduring assumptions’,
Australian Educational Researcher
, 42(2), pp. 155–77
Gordon, D., Adelman, L., Ashworth, K., Bradshaw, J., Levitas, R., Middleton, S., Pantazis, C., Patsios, D., Payne, S., Townsend, P. and
Williams, J. (2000)
Poverty and social exclusion in Britain.
York: JRF
Goulden, C. (2015) ‘Introduction: an anti-poverty strategy for the UK’,
Journal of Poverty and Social Justice
, 23(3), pp. 165–6
Goulden, C. and D'Arcy, C. (2014)
A definition of poverty.
York: JRF
Grebelsky-Lichtman, T. (2014) ‘Parental patterns of cooperation in parent-child interactions: the relationship between nonverbal
and verbal communication’,
Human Communication Research
, 40(1), pp. 1–29
Green, L., Fry, A. and Myerson, J. (1994) ‘Discounting of delayed rewards: a life-span comparison’,
Psychological Science
, 5(1), pp.
33–6
Greene, C. A. and Murdock, K. K. (2013) ‘Multidimensional control beliefs, socioeconomic status, and health’,
American Journal of
Health Behavior
, 37(2), pp. 227–37
53
Griskevicius, V., Tybur, J. M., Delton, A. W. and Robertson, T. E. (2011) ‘The influence of mortality and socioeconomic status on risk
and delayed rewards: a life history theory approach’,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
, 100(6), pp. 1015–26
Groffen, D. A. I., Bosma, H., Tan, F. E. S., van den Akker, M., Kempen, G. I. J. M. and van Eijk, J. T. M. (2012) ‘Material vs. psychosocial
explanations of old-age educational differences in physical and mental functioning’,
European Journal of Public Health
, 22(4),
pp. 587–92
Grotz, M., Hapke, U., Lampert, T. and Baumeister, H. (2011) ‘Health locus of control and health behaviour: results from a nationally
representative survey’,
Psychology, Health and Medicine,
16(2), pp. 129–40
Gutman, L. M. and Schoon, I. (2012) ‘Correlates and consequences of uncertainty in career aspirations: gender differences among
adolescents in England’,
Journal of Vocational Behavior
, 80(3), pp. 608–18
Guttersrud, Ø. (2015) ‘Young adolescents' engagement in dietary behaviour – the impact of gender, socio-economic status, self-
efficacy and scientific literacy. Methodological aspects of constructing measures in nutrition literacy research using the Rasch
model’,
Public Health Nutrition
, 18(14), pp. 2565–74
Hackman, D. A. and Farah, M. J. (2009) ‘Socioeconomic status and the developing brain’,
Trends in Cognitive Sciences
, 13(2), pp.
65–73
Haisley, E., Mostafa, R. and Loewenstein, G. (2008) ‘Myopic risk-seeking: the impact of narrow decision bracketing on lottery
play’,
Journal of Risk and Uncertainty
, 37(1), pp. 57–75
Hakulinen, C., Jokela, M., Hintsanen, M., Pulkki-Råback, L., Hintsa, T., Merjonen, P., Josefsson, K., Kähönen, M., Raitakari, O. T. and
Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. (2013) ‘Childhood family factors predict developmental trajectories of hostility and anger: a
prospective study from childhood into middle adulthood’,
Psychological Medicine
, 43(11), pp. 2417–26
Hall, E., Chai, W. and Albrecht, J. A. (2016) ‘Relationships between nutrition-related knowledge, self-efficacy, and behavior for fifth
grade students attending Title I and non-Title I schools’,
Appetite
, 96, pp. 245–53
Harknett, K. S. and Hartnett, C. S. (2011) ‘Who lacks support and why? An examination of mothers' personal safety net’,.
Journal of
Marriage and Family
, 73(4), pp. 861–75
Harrison, G. W., Morten, I. L. and Williams, M. B. (2002) ‘Estimating individual discount rates in Denmark’,
American Economic
Review
, 92
,
pp. 1606–17
Haushofer, J. and Fehr, E. (2014) ‘On the psychology of povert’,.
Science
, 344(6186), pp. 862–7
Henninger, W. R. I. V. and Gross, P. E. (2016) ‘Maternal warmth: a re-examination of the role race and socioeconomic status play’,
Journal of Ethnic & Cultural Diversity in Social Work,
25(1), pp. 36–49
Hogan, M. J., Parker, J. D. A., Wiener, J., Watters, C., Wood, L. M. and Oke, A. (2010) ‘Academic success in adolescence:
relationships among verbal IQ, social support and emotional intelligence’,
Australian Journal of Psychology
, 62(1), pp. 30–41
Holmes, J. and Kiernan, K. (2013) ‘Persistent poverty and children's development in the early years of childhood’,
Policy and politics
,
41(1), pp. 19–42
Hornstra, L., van der Veen, I., Peetsma, T. and Volman, M. (2013) ‘Developments in motivation and achievement during primary
school: a longitudinal study on group-specific differences’,
Learning and Individual Differences
, 23, pp. 195–204
Hornstra, L., van der Veen, I., Peetsma, T. and Volman, M. (2015) ‘Does classroom composition make a difference: effects on
developments in motivation, sense of classroom belonging and achievement in upper primary school’,
School Effectiveness
and School Improvement
, 26(2), pp. 125–52
Hughes, C., Ensor, R., Wilson, A. and Graham, A. (2010) ‘Tracking executive function across the transition to school: a latent variable
approach’,
Developmental Neuropsychology,
35(1), pp.20–36
Hyde, L. W., Shaw, D. S. and Moilanen, K. L. (2010) ‘Developmental precursors of moral disengagement and the role of moral
disengagement in the development of antisocial behavior’,
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology
, 38(2), pp. 197–209
James, S. L. and Amato, P. R. (2013) ‘Self-esteem and the reproduction of social class’,
Social Science Quarterly
, 94(5), pp. 933–55
Jaser, S. S., Faulkner, M. S., Whittemore, R., Jeon, S., Murphy, K., Delamater, A. and Grey, M. (2012) ‘Coping, self-management, and
adaptation in adolescents with type 1 diabetes’,
Annals Of Behavioral Medicine: A Publication Of The Society Of Behavioral
Medicine
, 43(3), pp. 311–9
Jin, H., Oshima, K. M. M. and Youngmi, K. (2010) ‘Does food insecurity affect parental characteristics and child behavior? Testing
nediation effects’,
Social Service Review
, 84(3), pp. 381–401
54
John-Henderson, N. A., Rheinschmidt, M. L., Mendoza-Denton, R. and Francis, D. D. (2014) ‘Performance and inflammation
outcomes are predicted by different facets of SES under stereotype threat’,
Social Psychological and Personality Science
,
5(3), pp. 301–09
Johnson, M. K. and Mortimer, J. T. (2011) ‘Origins and outcomes of judgments about work’,
Social Forces
, 89(4), pp. 1239–60
Johnson, S. B., Richeson, J. A. and Finkel, E. J. (2011) ‘Middle class and marginal? Socioeconomic status, stigma, and self-regulation
at an elite university’,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
, 100(5), pp. 838–52
Kalaycıoğlu, D. B. (2015) ‘The influence of socioeconomic status, self-efficacy, and anxiety on mathematics achievement in England,
Greece, Hong Kong, the Netherlands, Turkey, and the USA’,
Kuram ve Uygulamada Egitim Bilimleri
, 15(5), pp. 1391–1401
Kalogrides, D. and Ovink, S. (2013) ‘The role of social and behavioral skills in mediating achievement gaps in elementary school’,
Conference Papers–American Sociological Association
, pp. 1–43
Kan, C., Kawakami, N. and Umeda, M. (2015) ‘Mediating role of psychological resources on the association between childhood
socioeconomic status and current health in the community adult population of Japan’,
International Journal Of Behavioral
Medicine
, 22(6), pp. 764–74
Karaarslan, G. and Sungur, S. (2011) ‘Elementary students' self-efficacy beliefs in science: role of grade level, gender, and socio-
economic status’,
Science Education International
, 22(1), pp. 72–9
Karwowski, M. (2011.’ It doesn't hurt to ask… But sometimes it hurts to believe: Polish students' creative self-efficacy and its
predictors’,
Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts,
5(2), pp. 154–64
Katz, N. and Hartman-Maeir, A. (1997) ‘Occupational performance and metacognition’,
Canadian Journal of Occupational Therapy
,
64(2), pp. 53–62
Keltner, D., Gruenfeld, D. H. and Anderson, C. (2003) ‘Power, approach, and inhibition’,
Psychological Review
, 110(2), p. 265
Khurana, A., Romer, D., Betancourt, L. M., Brodsky, N. L., Giannetta, J. M. and Hurt, H. (2015) ‘Stronger working memory reduces
sexual risk taking in adolescents, even after controlling for parental influences’,
Child Development
, 86(4), pp. 1125–41
Kiviruusu, O., Huurre, T., Haukkala, A. and Aro, H. (2013) ‘Changes in psychological resources moderate the effect of socioeconomic
status on distress symptoms: a 10-year follow-up among young adults’,
Health Psychology
, 32(6), pp. 627–36
Knack, S. (2002) ‘Social capital, growth and poverty: a survey of cross-country evidence’. In C. Grootaert and T. van Bastelaer
(eds),
The role of social capital in development: an empirical assessment
. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Kraus, M. W., Horberg, E. J., Goetz, J. L. and Keltner, D. (2011) ‘Social class rank, threat vigilance, and hostile reactivity’,
Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin
, 37(10), pp. 1376–88
Kriegbaum, K. and Spinath, B. (2016) ‘Explaining social disparities in mathematical achievement: the role of motivation’,
European
Journal of Personality
, 30(1), pp. 45–63
Kushel, M. B., Gupta, R., Gee, L. and Haas, J. S. (2006) ‘Housing instability and food insecurity as barriers to health care among low
income Americans’,
Journal of General Internal Medicine,
21(1), pp. 71–7
Kwon, J. A. and Wickrama, K. A. S. (2014) ‘Linking family economic pressure and supportive parenting to adolescent health
behaviors: two developmental pathways leading to health promoting and health risk behaviors’,
Journal of Youth and
Adolescence
, 43(7), pp. 1176–90
Lachman, M. E., Weaver S. L. (1998) ‘The sense of control as a moderator of social class differences in health and well-
being’,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
, 74, pp. 763–73
Laurin, K., Fitzsimons, G. M. and Kay, A. C. (2011) ‘Social disadvantage and the self-regulatory function of justice beliefs’,
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology
, 100(1), pp. 149–71
Lawrence, E. (2015) ‘The family-school interaction: school composition and parental educational expectations in the United States’,
British Educational Research Journal
, 41(2), pp. 183–209
Lee, J. O., Hill, K. G. and Hawkins, J. D. (2012) ‘The role of educational aspirations and expectations in the discontinuity of
intergenerational low-income status’,
Social Work Research
, 36(2), pp. 141–51
Lehto, E., Konttinen, H., Jousilahti, P. and Haukkala, A. (2013) ‘The role of psychosocial factors in socioeconomic differences in
physical activity: a population-based study’,
Scandinavian Journal of Public Health
, 41(6), pp. 553–9
Lewis, O. (1996) ‘The culture of poverty’. In G. Gmelch and W. Zenner (eds),
Urban Life
. Long Grove, IL: Waveland Press
55
Lipina, S. J., Martelli, M. I., Vuelta, B. and Colombo, J. A. (2005) ‘Performance on the A-not-B task of Argentinean infants from
unsatisfied and satisfied basic needs homes’,
International Journal of Psychology
, 39, pp. 49–60
Livingston, M. (2014) ‘Socioeconomic differences in alcohol-related risk-taking behaviours’,
Drug and Alcohol Review
, 33(6), pp.
588–95
Loopstra, R., Reeves, A., Taylor-Robinson, D., Barr, B., McKee, M. and Stuckler, D. (2015) ‘Austerity, sanctions, and the rise of food
banks in the UK’
British Medical Journal
, 350, h1775
Lunn, P. and Lyons, S. (2010)
Behavioural economics and “vulnerable consumers”: a summary of evidence.
London:
Communications Consumer Panel
Lupien, S. J. King, S. Meaney, M. J. and McEwen, B. S. (2001) ‘Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive
function in children from low and high socioeconomic status’,
Developmental Psychopathology
, 13, pp. 651–74
Luthar, S. S. (2003)
Resilience and vulnerability: adaptation in the context of childhood adversities
. Cambridge: Cambridge University
Press
Lütke, N. and Lange-Küttner, C. (2015) ‘Keeping it in three dimensions: measuring the development of mental rotation in children
with the Rotated Colour Cube Test (RCCT)’,
International Journal of Developmental Sciences
, 9(2), pp. 95–114
Lyles, C. R., Wolf, M. S., Schillinger, D., Davis, T. C., Dewalt, D., Dahlke, A. R., Curtis, L. and Seligman, H. K. (2013) ‘Food insecurity in
relation to changes in hemoglobin a1c, self-efficacy, and fruit/vegetable intake during a diabetes educational intervention’,
Diabetes Care
, 36(6), pp. 1448–53
Lyman, E. L. and Luthar, S. S. (2014) ‘Further evidence on the 'costs of privilege': perfectionism in highachieving youth at
socioeconomic extremes’,
Psychology in the Schools
, 51(9), pp. 913–30
MacDonald, R., Shildrick, T. and Furlong, A. (2014) ‘'Benefits Street' and the myth of workless communities’,
Sociological Research
Online
, 19(3), p. 1
MacInnes, T., Tinson, A., Hughes,C., Born, T. B. and Aldridge , H. (2015)
Monitoring poverty and social exclusion 2015.
York: JRF
Madrian, B. C. (2014)
Applying insights from behavioral economics to policy design
. NBER Working Paper No. 20318. Cambridge,
MA: National Bureau of Economic Research
Maier, S. F. and Seligman, M. E. (1976) ‘Learned helplessness: theory and evidence’,
Journal of experimental psychology:
general
, 105(1), p. 3
Malti, T., Averdijk, M., Ribeaud, D., Rotenberg, K. and Eisner, M. (2013) ‘'Do you trust him?' Children's trust beliefs and developmental
trajectories of aggressive behavior in an ethnically diverse sample’,
Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology
, 41(3), pp. 445–56
Manganelli, S., Lucidi, F. and Alivernini, F. (2015) ‘Italian adolescents' civic engagement and open classroom climate: the mediating
role of self-efficacy’,
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
, 41, pp. 8–18
Mani, A., Mullainathan, S., Shafir, E. and Zhao, J. (2013) ‘Poverty impedes cognitive function’,
Science,
341(6149), pp. 976–80
Marcus Jenkins, J. V., Woolley, D. P., Hooper, S. R. and De Bellis, M. D. (2013) ‘Direct and indirect effects of brain volume,
socioeconomic status and family stress on child IQ’,
Journal Of Child And Adolescent Behavior
, 1(2)
Markant, J., Ackerman, L. K., Nussenbaum, K. and Amso, D. (2015) ‘Selective attention neutralizes the adverse effects of low
socioeconomic status on memory in 9-month-old infants’,
Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
, 18, pp. 26–33
Markovits, H. and Brunet, M.-L. (2012) ‘Priming divergent thinking promotes logical reasoning in 6- to 8-year olds: but more for
high than low SES students’,
Journal of Cognitive Psychology
, 24(8), pp. 991–1001
Martini, F. and Sénéchal, M. (2012) ‘Learning literacy skills at home: parent teaching, expectations, and child interest’,
Canadian
Journal of Behavioural Science
, 44(3), pp. 210–21
Matte-Gagné, C. and Bernier, A. (2011) ‘Prospective relations between maternal autonomy support and child executive functioning:
investigating the mediating role of child language ability’,
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
, 110(4), pp. 611–25
Maupin, A. N., BrophyHerb, H. E., Schiffman, R. F. and Bocknek, E. L. (2010) ‘Lowincome parental profiles of coping, resource
adequacy, and public assistance receipt: links to parenting’,
Family Relations
, 59(2), pp. 180–94
Mazur, J., Malkowska-Szkutnik, A. and Tabak, I. (2014) ‘Changes in family socio-economic status as predictors of self-efficacy in 13-
year-old Polish adolescents’,
International Journal of Public Health
, 59(1), pp. 107–15
56
McCoy, D. C., Connors, M. C., Morris, P. A., Yoshikawa, H. and Friedman-Krauss, A. H. (2015) ‘Neighborhood economic disadvantage
and children's cognitive and social-emotional development: exploring head start classroom quality as a mediating mechanism’,
Early Childhood Research Quarterly
, 32, pp. 150–59
McGeown, S. P., Putwain, D., Geijer Simpson, E., Boffey, E., Markham, J. and Vince, A. (2014) ‘Predictors of adolescents' academic
motivation: personality, self-efficacy and adolescents' characteristics’,
Learning and Individual Differences
, 32, pp. 278–86
McKendrick, J. H., Sinclair, S., Irwin, A., O’Donnell, H., Scott, G. and Dobbie, L. (2008)
The media, poverty and public opinion in the
UK
. York: JRF
Meisenberg, G. and Williams, A. (2008) ‘Are acquiescent and extreme response styles related to low intelligence and
education?’,
Personality and Individual Differences
, 44(7), pp. 1539–50
Merolla, D. M., Hunt, M. O. and Serpe, R. T. (2011) ‘Concentrated disadvantage and beliefs about the causes of poverty: a multi-level
analysis’,
Sociological Perspectives
, 54(2), pp. 205–27
Metcalfe, L. A., Harvey, E. A. and Laws, H. B. (2013) ‘The longitudinal relation between academic/cognitive skills and externalizing
behavior problems in preschool children’,
Journal of Educational Psychology
, 105(3), pp. 881–94
Mezzacappa, E. (2004) ‘Alerting, orienting, and executive attention: developmental properties and socio-demographic correlates in
an epidemiological sample of young, urban children’,
Child Development
,
75, pp. 1373–86
Mezzacappa, E., Buckner, J. C. and Earls, F. (2011) ‘Prenatal cigarette exposure and infant learning stimulation as predictors of
cognitive control in childhood’,
Developmental Science
, 14(4), pp. 881–91
Micha, R., Rogers, P. J. and Nelson, M. (2010) ‘The glycaemic potency of breakfast and cognitive function in school children’,
European Journal Of Clinical Nutrition
, 64(9), pp. 948–57
Mishra, S., Barclay, P. and Lalumière, M. L. (2014) ‘Competitive disadvantage facilitates risk-taking’,
Evolution and Human
Behavior
, 35, pp. 126–32
Mishra, S. and Lalumière, M.L. (2010) ‘You can’t always get what you want: the motivational effect of need on risk-sensitive
decision-making’,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
, 46(4), pp. 605–11
Mishra, S. and Novakowski, D. (2016) ‘Personal relative deprivation and risk: an examination of individual differences in personality,
attitudes, and behavioral outcomes’,
Personality and Individual Differences
,
90, pp. 22–6
Moilanen, K. L., Shaw, D. S., Dishion, T. J., Gardner, F. and Wilson, M. (2010) ‘Predictors of longitudinal growth in inhibitory control
in early childhood’,
Social Development
, 19(2), pp. 326–47
Moilanen, K. L. and Shen, Y.-L. (2014) ‘Mastery in middle adolescence: the contributions of socioeconomic status, maternal mastery
and supportive-involved mothering’,
Journal of Youth and Adolescence
, 43(2), pp. 298–310
Moscatelli, S., Albarello, F., Prati, F. and Rubini, M. (2014) ‘Badly off or better off than them? The impact of relative deprivation and
relative gratification on intergroup discrimination’,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
, 107(2), pp. 248–64
Moynihan, D. P. (1965)
The negro family: the case for national action.
Washington, DC: Office of Policy Planning and Research, US
Department of Labor
Mullainathan, S. and Shafir, E. (2013)
Scarcity: why having too little means so much
. New York: Macmillan
Murray, T. and Rodgers, W. (2012) ‘The role of socioeconomic status and control beliefs on frequency of exercise during and after
cardiac rehabilitation’,
Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being
, 4(1), pp. 49–66
Murray, T., Rodgers, W. and Fraser, S. (2012) ‘Exploring the relationship between socioeconomic status, control beliefs and exercise
behavior: a multiple mediator model’,
Journal of Behavioral Medicine
, 35(1), pp. 63–73
Musterd, S. (2005) ‘Social and ethnic segregation in Europe: levels, causes, and effects’,
Journal of Urban Affairs
, 27(3), pp. 331–48
Nelson, C. A. and Sheridan, M. A. (2011) ‘Lessons from neuroscience research for understanding causal links between family and
neighborhood characteristics and educational outcomes’. In G. J. Duncan and R. J. Murnane (eds),
Whither opportunity?
New
York: Russell Sage Foundation
Neumann, A., Barker, E. D., Koot, H. M. and Maughan, B. (2010) ‘The role of contextual risk, impulsivity, and parental knowledge in
the development of adolescent antisocial behavior’,
Journal of Abnormal Psychology
, 119(3), pp. 534–45
Newland, R. P., Crnic, K. A., Cox, M. J. and Mills-Koonce, W. R. (2013) ‘The family model stress and maternal psychological
symptoms: mediated pathways from economic hardship to parenting’,
Journal of Family Psychology
, 27(1), pp. 96–105
57
Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., Akshoomoff, N., Amaral, D. G., Bloss, C. S., Libiger, O.,
Schork, N. J., Murray, S. S., Casey, B. J., Chang, L., Ernst, T. M., Frazier, J. A., Gruen, J. R., Kennedy, D. N., Van Zijl, P., Mostofsky,
S., Kaufmann, W. E., Kenet, T., Dale, A. M., Jernigan, T. L. and Sowell, E. R. (2015) ‘Family income, parental education and brain
structure in children and adolescents’,
Nature Neuroscience
, 18(5), pp. 773–8
Novakovic, A. and Fouad, N. A. (2013) ‘Background, personal, and environmental influences on the career planning of adolescent
girls’,
Journal of Career Development
, 40(3), pp. 223–44
Odgers, C. L., Caspi, A., Russell, M. A., Sampson, R. J., Arseneault, L. and Moffit, T. E. (2012) ‘Supportive parenting mediates
neighborhood socioeconomic disparities in children's antisocial behavior from ages 5–12’,
Development and
Psychopathology
, 24(3), pp. 705–21
OECD (2016)
List of OECD member countries – ratification of the Convention on the OECD
. Available
from: http://bit.ly/1hnDVqG [accessed 17 October 2016]
Oyserman, D., Johnson, E., James, L. (2011) ‘Seeing the destination but not the path: effects of socioeconomic disadvantage on
school-focused possible self content and linked behavioral strategies’,
Self and Identity
, 10(4), pp. 474–92
Packard, C. J., Cavanagh, J., McLean, J. S., McConnachie, A., Messow, C.-M., Batty, G. D., Burns, H., Deans, K. A., Sattar, N., Shiels, P.
G., Velupillai, Y. N., Tannahill, C. and Millar, K. (2012) ‘Interaction of personality traits with social deprivation in determining
mental wellbeing and health behaviours’,
Journal Of Public Health
, 34(4), pp. 615–24
Pampel, F. C., Krueger, P. M. and Denney, J. T. (2010) ‘Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviours’,
Annual Review of Sociology
,
36, p. 349
Parr, A. K. and Bonitz, V. S. (2015) ‘Role of family background, student behaviors, and school-related beliefs in predicting high
school dropout’,
Journal of Educational Research
, 108(6), pp. 504–14
Paulhus, D. L. and Williams, K. M. (2002) ‘The dark triad of personality: narcissism, Machiavellianism and psychopathy’,
Journal of
Research in Personality
, 36, pp. 556–63
Pepper, G. V. and Nettle, D. (2014) ‘Socioeconomic disparities in health behaviour: an evolutionary perspective’. In D. W. Lawson
and M. Gibson (eds),
Applied evolutionary anthropology: Darwinian approaches to contemporary world issues
. New York:
Springer-Verlag
Pepper, G. V. and Nettle, D. (under review) ‘The behavioural constellation of deprivation: causes and consequences’,
Behavioral and
Brain Sciences
Pereira, M., Negrão, M., Soares, I. and Mesman, J. (2015) ‘Predicting harsh discipline in at-risk mothers: the moderating effect of
socioeconomic deprivation severity’,
Journal of Child and Family Studies
, 24(3), pp. 725–33
Peretti-Watel, P., L’Haridon, O. and Seror, V. (2013) ‘Time preferences, socioeconomic status and smokers' behaviour, attitudes and
risk awareness’,
European Journal of Public Health
, 23(5), pp. 783–8
Perry, B. L., Link, T., Boelter, C. and Leukefeld, C. (2012) ‘Blinded to science: gender differences in the effects of race, ethnicity, and
socioeconomic status on academic and science attitudes among sixth graders’,
Gender and Education
, 24(7), pp. 725–43
Petersen, J. and Lindström, M. (2010) ‘Materialist/postmaterialist values and their association with psychological health and health
locus of control: a population-based study’,
Social Science Journal
, 47(4), pp. 789–801
Piff, P. K., Kraus, M. W., Côté, S., Cheng, B. H. and Keltner, D. (2010) ‘Having less, giving more: the influence of social class on
prosocial behavior’,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
99(5), pp. 771–84
Pina, V., Fuentes, L. J., Castillo, A. and Diamantopoulou, S. (2014) ‘Disentangling the effects of working memory, language, parental
education, and non-verbal intelligence on children's mathematical abilities’,
Frontiers in Psychology
, 5, Article 415
Piotrowski, J., Lapierre, M. and Linebarger, D. (2013) ‘Investigating correlates of self-regulation in early childhood with a
representative sample of English-speaking American families’,
Journal of Child and Family Studies
, 22(3), pp. 423–36
Pittman, T. S. and D'Agostino, P. R. (1989) ‘Motivation and cognition: control deprivation and the nature of subsequent information
processing’,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology
, 25(6), pp. 465–80
Prawitz, A. D., Kalkowski, J. C. and Cohart, J. (2013) ‘Responses to economic pressure by low-income families: financial distress and
hopefulness’,
Journal of Family and Economic Issues
, 34(1), pp. 29–40
Quon, E. C. and McGrath, J. J. (2015) ‘Community, family, and subjective socioeconomic status: relative status and adolescent
health’,
Health Psychology
, 34(6), pp. 591–601
58
Rajchert, J. M., Zułtak, T. and Smulczyk, M. (2014) ‘Predicting reading literacy and its improvement in the Polish national extension
of the PISA study: the role of intelligence, trait- and state-anxiety, socio-economic status and school-type’,
Learning and
Individual Differences
, 33, pp. 1–11
Raver, C. C., Blair, C. and Garrett-Peters, P. (2015) ‘Poverty, household chaos, and interparental aggression predict children's ability
to recognize and modulate negative emotions’,
Development and Psychopathology
, 27(3), pp. 695–708
Raver, C. C., Blair, C. and Willoughby, M. (2013) ‘Poverty as a predictor of 4-year-olds' executive function: new perspectives on
models of differential susceptibility’,
Developmental Psychology
, 49(2), pp. 292–304
Razza, R. A., Martin, A. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2010) ‘Associations among family environment, sustained attention, and school
readiness for low-income children’,
Developmental Psychology
, 46(6), pp. 1528–42
Razza, R. A., Martin, A. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2012) ‘The implications of early attentional regulation for school success among low-
income children’,
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology,
33(6), 311-319
Rea, J. (2016)
Differences in values, mental health, self-mastery and social support between homeless, low socioeconomic status
and more affluent people: implications for social integration
. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Royal Holloway, University of
London
Ree, E., Odeen, M., Eriksen, H., Indahl, A., Ihlebæk, C., Hetland, J. and Harris, A. (2014) ‘Subjective health complaints and self-rated
health: are expectancies more important than socioeconomic status and workload?’,
International Journal of Behavioral
Medicine
, 21(3), pp. 411–20
Reid, J. L. and Ready, D. D. (2013) ‘High-quality preschool: the socioeconomic composition of preschool classrooms and children's
learning’,
Early Education and Development
, 24(8), pp. 1082–1111
Rew, L., Carver, T., Li, C-C. (2011) ‘Early and risky sexual behavior in a sample of rural adolescents’,
Issues in Comprehensive
Pediatric Nursing
, 34(4), pp. 189–204
Rigoli, D., Piek, J. P., Kane, R. and Oosterlaan, J. (2012) ‘Motor coordination, working memory, and academic achievement in a
normative adolescent sample: testing a mediation model’,
Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology
, 27(7), pp. 766–80
Ritchie, C. and Buchanan, A. (2010) ‘Self-report of parenting style, socio-economic status and psychological functioning in a
community sample of 13–15-year-old students’,
Journal of Social Work
, 10(3), pp. 317–32
Rochette, É. and Bernier, A. (2014) ‘Parenting, family ocioeconomic status, and child executive functioning: a longitudinal study’,
Merrill-Palmer Quarterly
, 60(4), pp. 431–60
Roncancio, A. M., Berenson, A. B. and Rahman, M. (2012) ‘Health locus of control, acculturation, and health-related internet use
among Latinas’,
Journal of Health Communication
, 17(6), pp. 631–40
Rothbaum, F., Weisz, J. R. and Snyder, S. S. (1982) ‘Changing the world and changing the self: a two-process model of perceived
control’,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
, 42(1), p. 5
Rotter, J. B. (1966) ‘Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement’,
Journal of Educational
Research
, 74(3), pp. 185–90
Roy, A. L., McCoy, D. C. and Raver, C. C. (2014) ‘Instability versus quality: residential mobility, neighborhood poverty, and children's
self-regulation’,
Developmental Psychology
, 50(7), pp. 1891–6
Roze, E., Meijer, L., Van Braeckel, K. N. J. A., Ruiter, S. A. J., Bruggink, J. L. M. and Bos, A. F. (2010) ‘Developmental trajectories from
birth to school age in healthy term-born children’,
Pediatrics
, 126(5), pp. e1134–42
Ruberry, E. J., Lengua, L. J., Crocker, L. H., Bruce, J., Upshaw, M. B. and Sommerville, J. A. (2016) ‘Income, neural executive
processes, and preschool children's executive control’,
Development and Psychopathology
, pp. 1–12
Rubin, M. and Hewstone, M. (1998) ‘Social identity theory's self-esteem hypothesis: a review and some suggestions for
clarification;,
Personality and Social Psychology Review
, 2(1), pp. 40–62
Rucker, D. D. and Galinsky, A. D. (2008) ‘Desire to acquire: powerlessness and compensatory consumption’,
Journal of Consumer
Research,
35, pp. 257–67
Rucker, D. D. and Galinsky, A. D. (2009) ‘Conspicuous consumption versus utilitarian ideals: how different levels of power shape
consumer behaviour’,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
45(3), pp. 549–55
Salinas-Jiménez, M. d. M., Artés, J. and Salinas-Jiménez, J. (2010) ‘Income, motivation, and satisfaction with life: an empirical
analysis’,
Journal of Happiness Studies
, 11(6), pp. 779–93
59
Sampson, R. J. (2006) ‘Collective efficacy theory: lessons learned and directions for future inquiry’. In F. T. Cullen, J. Wright and K.
Blevins (eds),
Taking stock: the status of criminological theory
. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers
Sampson, R. J. (2012)
Great American city: Chicago and the enduring neighborhood effect
. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Sánchez-Pérez, N., Fuentes, L. J., Jolliffe, D. and González-Salinas, C. (2014) ‘Assessing children’s empathy through a Spanish
adaptation of the Basic Empathy Scale: parent’s and child’s report forms’,
Frontiers in Psychology
, 5, Article 1438
Santiago, C. D., Wadsworth, M. E. and Stump, J. (2011) ‘Socioeconomic status, neighborhood disadvantage, and poverty-related
stress: prospective effects on psychological syndromes among diverse low-income families’,
Journal of Economic Psychology
,
32(2), pp. 218–30
Sarsour, K., Sheridan, M., Jutte, D., Nuru-Jeter, A., Hinshaw, S. and Boyce, W. T. (2011) ‘Family socioeconomic status and child
executive functions: the roles of language, home environment, and single parenthood’,
Journal of the International
Neuropsychological Society
, 17(1), pp. 120–32
Schieman, S. and Narisada, A. (2014) ‘In control or fatalistically ruled? The sense of mastery among working Canadians’,
Canadian
Review of Sociology
, 51(4), pp. 343–74
Schofield, T. J., Martin, M. J., Conger, K. J., Neppl, T. M., Donnellan, M. B. and Conger, R. D. (2011) ‘Intergenerational transmission of
adaptive functioning: a test of the interactionist model of SES and human development’,
Child Development
, 82(1), pp. 33–47
Schoon, I. (2014) ‘Parental worklessness and the experience of NEET among their offspring. Evidence from the Longitudinal Study
of Young People in England (LSYPE)’,
Longitudinal and Life Course Studies
, 5(2), pp. 129–50
Schoon, I., Cheng, H., Gale, C. R., Batty, G. D. and Deary, I. J. (2010) ‘Social status, cognitive ability, and educational attainment as
predictors of liberal social attitudes and political trust’,
Intelligence,
38(1), pp. 144–50
Schoppe-Sullivan, S. J. and Mangelsdorf, S. C. (2013) ‘Parent characteristics and early coparenting behavior at the transition to
parenthood’,
Social Development
, 22(2), pp. 363–83
Schwartz, S. H. (1992) ‘Universals in the content and structure of values: theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries’,
Advances in Experimental Social Psychology
, 25, pp. 1–65
Sciarra, D. T. (2010) ‘Predictive factors in intensive math course–taking in high school’,
Professional School Counseling
, 13(3), pp.
196–207
Seeman, T. E. (1991) ‘Personal control and coronary artery disease: how generalized expectancies about control may influence
disease risk’,
Journal of Psychosomatic Research
, 35(6), pp. 661–9
Seeman, M. (2008) ‘Personal control’,
MacArthur Research Network on SES and Health Psychosocial Notebook
. Available from:
http://www.macses.ucsf.edu/research/psychosocial/control.php [accessed 17 October 2016]
Seeman, M., Stein Merkin, S., Karlamangla, A., Koretz, B. and Seeman, T. (2014) ‘Social status and biological dysregulation: the “status
syndrome” and allostatic load’,
Social Science and Medicine
, 118, pp. 143–51
Seligman, H. K., Davis, T. C., Schillinger, D. and Wolf, M. S. (2010) ‘Food insecurity is associated with hypoglycemia and poor diabetes
self-management in a low-income sample with diabetes’,
Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved
, 21(4), pp.
1227–33
Serbin, L. A., Temcheff, C. E., Cooperman, J. M., Stack, D. M., Ledingham, J. and Schwartzman, A. E. (2011) ‘Predicting family poverty
and other disadvantaged conditions for child rearing from childhood aggression and social withdrawal: a 30-year longitudinal
study’,
International Journal of Behavioral Development
, 35(2), pp. 97–106
Shah, A. K., Mullainathan, S. and Shafir, E. (2012) ‘Some consequences of having too little’,
Science,
338(6107), pp. 682–5
Sharma, E., Mazar, N., Alter, A. L. and Ariely, D. (2014) ‘Financial deprivation selectively shifts moral standards and compromises
moral decisions’,
Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
, 123(2), pp. 90–100
Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (2015) ‘Behavioural insights in the age of austerity: how the new ‘psychology of poverty’ can help us to stay
focused on society’,
Angle Journal
, 13
Sheehy-Skeffington, J. (in press) ‘Decision-making up against the wall: a framework for understanding the behavioural dimension of
low socioeconomic status’. In S. Oishi and A. Uskul (eds),
The Socioeconomic Environment and Human Psychology.
Oxford: Oxford University Press
Sheehy-Skeffington, J. and Haushofer, J. (2014) ‘The behavioural economics of poverty’,
Barriers and opportunities at the base of
the pyramid: prospects for private sector led-interventions
. Istanbul: UNDP Istanbul International Center for Private Sector in
Development
60
Sheehy-Skeffington, J. and Sidanius, J. (2015) ‘Out of my hands: low socioeconomic status diminishes personal sense of
control’
.
Paper presented at the British Psychological Society Developmental Section and Social Section Annual Conference,
Manchester, UK
Sheehy-Skeffington, J., Sidanius, J. and Price, M. E. (2016) ‘Decision-making at the bottom of the hierarchy: the cognitive impact of
perceiving oneself as low in socioeconomic status’. Flash-talk delivered to the Society of Personality and Social Psychology
Pre-Conference on the Emerging Psychology of Social Class, San Diego, CA
Shobe, M. A., Christy, K., Hamilton, L., Murphy-Erby, Y., Denny, G. S. and Givens, A. (2013) ‘Examining relationships between
financial capital and personal well-being’,
Journal of Poverty
, 17(1), pp. 47–62
Sivanathan, N. and Pettit, N. C. (2010) ‘Protecting the self through consumption: status goods as affirmational commodities’,
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology,
46(3), pp. 564–70
Sletten, M. A. (2011) ‘Limited expectations? How 14–16-year-old Norwegians in poor families look at their future’,
Young
, 19(2),
pp. 181–218
Slocum, L. A. E. E., Taylor, T. J., Brick, B. T. and Esbensen, F.-A. (2010) ‘Neighbourhood structural characteristics, individual-level
attitudes, and yourth's crime reporting intentions’,
Criminology,
48, pp. 1063–1100
Smeding, A., Darnon, C., Souchal, C., Toczek-Capelle, M. C. and Butera, F. (2013) ‘Reducing the socio-economic status achievement
gap at university by promoting mastery-oriented assessment’,
PLoS ONE
, 8(8), e71678
Smith, C. L. (2010) ‘Multiple determinants of parenting: predicting individual differences in maternal parenting behavior with
toddlers’,
Parenting: Science and Practice
, 10(1), pp. 1–17
Sortheix, F. M., Chow, A. and Salmela-Aro, K. (2015) ‘Work values and the transition to work life: a longitudinal study’,
Journal of
Vocational Behavior
, 89, pp. 162–71
South, S. J. and Crowder, K. D. (1997) ‘Escaping distressed neighborhoods: Individual, community, and metropolitan influences’,
American Journal of Sociology
, 102(4), pp. 1040–84
Speer, P., Peterson, N., Armstead, T. and Allen, C. (2013) ‘The influence of participation, gender and organizational sense of
community on psychological empowerment: the moderating effects of income’,
American Journal of Community Psychology
,
51(1/2), pp. 103–13
Spielberg, J. M., Galarce, E. M., Ladouceur, C. D., McMakin, D. L., Olino, T. M., Forbes, E. E., Silk, J. S., Ryan, N. D. and Dahl, R. E.
(2015) ‘Adolescent development of inhibition as a function of SES and gender: converging evidence from behavior and fMRI’,
Human Brain Mapping
, 36(8), pp. 3194–3203
Steele, C. M. and Sherman, D. A. (1999) ‘The psychological predicament of women on welfare’. In D. Prentice and D. Miller (eds)
,
Cultural divides: understanding and overcoming group conflict
. New York: Russell Sage Foundation
Steinmayr, R., Dinger, F. C. and Spinath, B. (2012) ‘Motivation as a mediator of social disparities in academic achievement’,
European
Journal of Personality
, 26(3), pp. 335–49
Stellar, J. E., Manzo, V. M., Kraus, M. W. and Keltner, D. (2012) ‘Class and compassion: socioeconomic factors predict responses to
suffering’,
Emotion
, 12(3), p. 449
Stephens, N. M., Fryberg, S. A. and Markus, H. R. (2011) ‘When choice does not equal
freedom: a sociocultural analysis of agency in working-class American contexts’,
Social Psychological and Personality Science
, 2(1), pp. 33–41
Stephens, N. M., Fryberg, S. A., Markus, H. R., Johnson, C. S. and Covarrubias, R. (2012) ‘Unseen disadvantage: how American
universities' focus on independence undermines the academic performance of first-generation college students’,
Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology
, 102(6), p. 1178
Stephens, N. M., Markus, H. R. and Phillips, L. T. (2014) ‘Social class culture cycles: how three gateway contexts shape selves and fuel
inequality’,
Annual Review of Psychology
, 65, pp. 611–34
Størksen, I., Ellingsen, I. T., Wanless, S. B. and McClelland, M. M. (2015) ‘The influence of parental socioeconomic background and
gender on self-regulation among 5-year-old children in Norway’,
Early Education and Development
, 26(5–6), pp. 663–84
Stringhini, S., Sabia, S., Shipley, M., Brunner, E., Nabi, H., Kivimaki, M. and Singh-Manoux, A. (2010) ‘Association of socioeconomic
position with health behaviors and mortality’,
Journal of the American
Medical Association
, 303(12), pp. 1159–66
Szepsenwol, O., Simpson, J. A., Griskevicius, V. and Raby, K. L. (2015) ‘The effect of unpredictable early childhood environments on
parenting in adulthood’,
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology
, 109(6), pp. 1045–67
61
Tabri, N., Dupuis, D. R., Kim, H. S. and Wohl, M. J. A. (2015) ‘Economic mobility moderates the effect of relative deprivation on
financial gambling motives and disordered gambling’,
International Gambling Studies
, 15(2), pp. 309–23
Tajfel, H. and Turner, J. C. (1979) ‘An integrative theory of intergroup conflict’,
The Social Psychology of Intergroup
Relations
, 33(47), p. 74
Tan, J. J. X. and Kraus, M. W. (2015) ‘Lay theories about social class buffer lower-class individuals against poor self-rated health and
negative affect’,
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin,
41(3), pp. 446–61
Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., Yu, Q., Sui, D., Rothbart, M. K., Fan, M. and Posner, M. I. (2007) ‘Short-term
meditation training improves attention and self-regulation’,
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
, 104(43), pp.
17,152–6
Tazouti, Y. and Jarlégan, A. (2014) ‘Socio-economic status, parenting practices and early learning at French kindergartens’,
International Journal of Early Years Education
, 22(3), pp. 287–300
Thompson, M. S., Thomas, M. E. and Head, R. N. (2012) ‘Race, socioeconomic status, and self-esteem: the impact of religiosity’,
Sociological Spectrum
, 32(5), pp. 385–405
Thornberry, T. P. (2003)
Gangs and delinquency in developmental perspective
. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Tikkanen, J. (2016) ‘Concern or confidence? Adolescents' identity capital and future worry in different school contexts’,
Journal of
Adolescence
, 46, pp. 14–24
Torres, D. D. (2013) ‘Understanding how family socioeconomic status mediates the maternal intelligence–child cognitive outcomes
relationship: a moderated mediation analysis’,
Biodemography and Social Biology
, 59(2), pp. 157–77
Trail, T. E. and Karney, B. R. (2012) ‘What's (not) wrong with low-income marriages’,
Journal of Marriage and Family
, 74(3), pp.
413–27
Trautmann, S. T., van de Kuilen, G. and Zeckhauser, R. J. (2013) ‘Social class and (un)ethical behavior: a framework, with evidence
from a large population sample’,
Perspectives on Psychological Science
, 8(5), pp. 487–97
Trentacosta, C. J., Donnellan, M. B., Shaw, D. S., Neppl, T. K., Scaramella, L. V. and Conger, R. D. (2010) ‘Adolescent personality as a
prospective predictor of parenting: an interactionist perspective’,
Journal of Family Psychology
, 24(6), pp. 721–30
Trope, Y. and Liberman, N. (2010) ‘Construal-level theory of psychological distance’,
Psychological Review
, 117(2), p. 440
Tucker-Drob, E. M. and Briley, D. A. (2012) ‘Socioeconomic status modifies interest-knowledge associations among adolescents’,
Personality and Individual Differences
, 53(1), pp. 9–15
Tynkkynen, L., Vuori, J. and Salmela-Aro, K. (2012) ‘The role of psychological control, socioeconomic status and academic
achievement in parents' educational aspirations for their adolescent children’,
European Journal of Developmental
Psychology
, 9(6), pp. 695–710
Umberson, D., Chen, M. D., House, J. S., Hopkins, K. and Slaten, E. (1996) ‘The effect of social relationships on psychological well-
being: are men and women really so different?’,
American Sociological Review
, 61(5), pp. 837–57
Uz Bas, A. and Yurdabakan, I. (2012) ‘Factor structure of the reactive-proactive aggression questionnaire in Turkish children and
gender, grade-level, and socioeconomic status differences in reactive and proactive aggression’,
Journal of Psychoeducational
Assessment,
30(3), pp. 284–97
Vandenbroucke, L., Verschueren, K., Ceulemans, E., De Smedt, B., De Roover, K. and Baeyens, D. (2015) ‘Family demographic
profiles and their relationship with the quality of executive functioning subcomponents in kindergarten’,
British Journal of
Developmental Psychology,
34(2), pp. 226–44
Vekiri, I. (2010) ‘Socioeconomic differences in elementary students ICT beliefs and out-of-school experiences’,
Computers and
Education
, 54(4), pp. 941–50
Veland, J., Bru, E. and Idsøe, T. (2015) ‘Perceived socio-economic status and social inclusion in school: parental monitoring and
support as mediators’,
Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties
, 20(2), pp. 173–88
Véronneau, M.-H., Serbin, L. A., Stack, D. M., Ledingham, J. and Schwartzman, A. E. (2015) ‘Emerging psychopathology moderates
upward social mobility: the intergenerational (dis)continuity of socioeconomic status’,
Development and Psychopathology
,
27(4, Pt 1), pp. 1217–36
Vettenburg, N., Brondeel, R., Gavray, C. and Pauwels, L. J. R. (2013) ‘Societal vulnerability and adolescent offending: the role of
violent values, self-control and troublesome youth group involvement’,
European Journal of Criminology
, 10(4), pp. 444–61
62
Vijayaraghavan, M., Jacobs, E. A., Seligman, H. and Fernandez, A. (2011) ‘The association between housing instability, food insecurity,
and diabetes self-efficacy in low-income adults’,
Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Underserved
, 22(4), pp. 1279–91
Vohs, K. D. and Baumeister, R. F. (eds) (2011)
Handbook of self-regulation: research, theory, and applications
. New York: Guilford
Press
von Stumm, S. (2012) ‘You are what you eat? Meal type, socio-economic status and cognitive ability in childhood’,
Intelligence
,
40(6), pp. 576–83
Von Stumm, S., Deary, I. J. and Hagger-Johnson, G. (2013) ‘Life-course pathways to psychological distress: a cohort study’,
BMJ
Open
, 3(5), p. e002772
von Stumm, S. and Plomin, R. (2015) ‘Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through
adolescence’,
Intelligence
, 48, pp. 30–36
Wadsworth, M. E., Rindlaub, L., Hurwich-Reiss, E., Rienks, S., Bianco, H. and Markman, H. J. (2013) ‘A longitudinal examination of the
Adaptation to Poverty-Related Stress Model: predicting child and adolescent adjustment over time’,
Journal of Clinical Child
and Adolescent Psychology,
42(5), pp. 713–25
Wagner, U. and Zick, A. (1995) ‘The relation of formal education to ethnic prejudice: its reliability, validity and
explanation’,
European Journal of Social Psychology
, 25(1), pp. 41–56
Walker, A. F., Schatz, D. A., Johnson, C., Silverstein, J. H. and Rohrs, H. J. (2015) ‘Disparities in social support systems for youths with
Type 1 diabetes’,
Clinical Diabetes
, 33(2), pp. 62–9
Ward, M. M. (2013) ‘Parental educational attainment and sense of control in mid- and late-adulthood’,
Developmental Psychology
,
49(7), pp. 1407–12
Warner, T., Swisher, R., Chavez, J. and Kuhl, D. (2011) ‘Contextualizing multiple dimensions of structural inequality: a neighborhood-
centered approach to place’,
Conference Papers –American Sociological Association
Wiederkehr, V., Bonnot, V., Krauth-Gruber, S. and Darnon, C. (2015a) ‘Belief in school meritocracy as a system-justifying tool for
low status students’,
Frontiers in Psychology
, 6, Article 1053
Wiederkehr, V., Darnon, C., Chazal, S., Guimond, S. and Martinot, D. (2015b) ‘From social class to self-efficacy: internalization of low
social status pupils' school performance’,
Social Psychology of Education
, 18(4), pp. 769–84
Wolniak, G. and Rekoutis, P. (2016) ‘Factors associated with college coping among high-achieving scholarship recipients from
adverse backgrounds’,
Teachers College Record,
118(1), pp. 1–32
World Bank (2014)
World Development Report 2015: mind, society, and behavior
. Washington, D. C.: World Bank
Wrulich, M., Brunner, M., Stadler, G., Schalke, D., Keller, U., Chmiel, M. and Martin, R. (2013) ‘Childhood intelligence and adult health:
the mediating roles of education and socioeconomic status’,
Intelligence
, 41(5), pp. 490–500
Yerdelen-Damar, S. and Pesman, H. (2013) ‘Relations of gender and socioeconomic status to physics through metacognition and
self-efficacy’,
Journal of Educational Research
, 106(4), pp. 280–89
Zarnowiecki, D., Sinn, N., Petkov, J. and Dollman, J. (2012) ‘Parental nutrition knowledge and attitudes as predictors of 5–6-year-
old children's healthy food knowledge’,
Public Health Nutrition,
15(7), pp. 1284–90
Zelazo, P. D., Carter, A., Reznick, J. S. and Frye, D. (1997) ‘Early development of executive function: a problem-solving
framework’,
Review of General Psychology
, 1(2), p. 198
Zhao, J., Cheng, J., Harris, M. and Vigo, R. (2015) ‘Anxiety and intertemporal decision making: the effect of the behavioral inhibition
system and the moderation effects of trait anxiety on both state anxiety and socioeconomic status’,
Personality and Individual
Differences
, 87, pp. 236–41
Zilanawala, A. and Pilkauskas, N. (2011) ‘Low-income mothers' material hardship and children's socioemotional wellbeing’,
Conference Papers – American Sociological Association
63
Appendix
Introduction and objectives of the review
This project aimed to understand what is known about how living in poverty influences individual
decision-making within high-income nations, and what influences the capacity, ability and choice of those
affected by poverty. The authors looked specifically at the evidence regarding the relationship between
socioeconomic status (SES) and the psychological, social and cultural processes that underpin decision-
making. In referring to these processes as psychosocial mechanisms, the project investigated the extent
to which they mediate the influence of poverty on the kinds of decisions and behaviour that are observed
more commonly in low-income than high-income groups. The shift in focus from context-specific
decision domains (e.g. education, family and finance) to the general mechanisms that typically inform
these decisions (e.g. risk-taking or personal value orientations) enables the design of interventions that
can have a more effective and wide-ranging impact. The project was sensitive to the purposive and often
adaptive ways in which people respond to the situation of poverty, and to how their decisions can be
framed as addressing proximal functions, though often at the cost of distal goals.
In this research, poverty was not only operationalised as a financial situation, but also in terms of its
associated experiences, such as being deprived, being low in relative SES, and being marginalised and
socially excluded. The set of psychosocial processes considered was also broadly construed, and narrowed
down in a systematic manner as the literature search progressed. Some 54 different Boolean searches of
the literature were conducted, which ultimately resulted in 15 related systematic reviews, with the same
set of filtering criteria being applied to each. A full discussion of the rationale for the project, the context
within which the research question was formulated, results of the reviews, and implication lessons for
policies and interventions designed to reduce poverty can be found in the main report. This Appendix is
intended to provide additional information with regard to the methodological rationale and decisions
taken in the systematic review process. The information herein is presented in accordance with the
guidelines of the Campbell Collaboration (2001) as far as is practical without producing unnecessary
duplication of content in the primary synthesis report.
Methodology
Criteria for relevance
This review concerned itself with how living in poverty influences individual decision-making within high-
income nations. In order to include ecologically valid findings, only those studies that were conducted in
societies with applicable socioeconomic environments were included. To this end, only OECD nations
(OECD, 2016) excluding Chile and Mexico were included, and this decision was taken prior to conducting
the search (details of the exclusion Boolean can be found in Table 1). Further criteria for relevance were
determined following a pilot search excluding non-OECD nations. The details of the final set of exclusion
criteria can be found in Table 2, and the respective screening and filtering of papers, to arrive at the final
list for inclusion, can be found in Table 3.
64
Table 1: Boolean search terms by reported mechanism
65
66
Table 2: Exclusion criteria
67
Table 3: Screening and filtering process
68
Relevance decisions were made at the following levels by a research assistant (RA1) under the oversight
of the principal investigator (PI) and the research associate (RA2). Deleted items were also scanned by a
different researcher from the person who did the scanning. Deleted papers that appeared potentially
relevant were reintroduced to the literature for more thorough screening to reduce the margin of error.
Table 4: Relevance decisions
Search strategy for identification of relevant studies
The following sources of information were searched for relevant content spanning the fields of
psychology, economics, sociology, anthropology, political science, education, medicine, and the social
sciences more generally. These databases were searched for journal articles, conference proceedings and
dissertations published between January 2010 and January 2016. Boolean operators were included for
poverty-related search terms ‘and’ mechanism-related search terms with additional ‘not’ Boolean to
remove non-human and non-OECD samples. There were a total of 54 mechanisms included in the initial
search. The list of mechanisms was developed from a combination of psychological theory and
consideration of the existing literature, and was intended to cover all plausible high-level processes that
might underlie decision-making and behaviour. The full list of mechanisms used in the initial search, the
results of which constituted the long list, can be found in Table 5. Detailed Boolean commands for the
poverty-related terms, the exclusion search terms and the final set of mechanisms can be found in Table
1. Boolean commands used for the remaining mechanism searches are available on request from the
authors.
69
Table 5: Additional mechanisms in the exploratory list
Published articles, conference proceedings, theses and dissertations
EBSCO: Anthropology Plus, British Education Index, Business Source Complete, Child Development
and Adolescent Studies, Criminal Justice Abstracts with Full Text, Econ Lit, ERIC (education
database), International Political Science Abstracts, MEDLINE, PsycARTICLES, PsycINFO, SocINDEX.
IBSS: all subject disciplines searched including Anthropology, Economics, Political and Sociology.
70
Scopus: Social Sciences and Humanities database; Scopus only enabled the import of 2,000
references, and therefore the 2,000 most recent references were included for each of the individual
searches.
Conference proceedings, theses and dissertations
Web of Science: Science, Social Sciences and Humanities were searched for
Title/Topic/Conference.
Zetoc: Conference proceedings with each individual Boolean based on automated AND statements.
Proquest: Masters and PhD dissertations searched.
Grey literature (other)
Charity reports: End Child Poverty, Oxfam, Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
Individual contacts: 152 researchers in the broad area of poverty and decision-making.
Research networks: including SPSSI, APA, LSE, Washington, poverty.ac.uk.
Listservs: h-net, SPSP, EASP, JDMS.
Social networking: ResearchGate topic calls (# of followers): Decision-making (6,024), Poverty
(3,744), Cultural Psychology (497), Socioeconomic status (39), Socioeconomic factors (549),
Judgement and decision making (493), Decision making under uncertainty (1,109), Socio-cultural
(1,575), Social exclusion (4,587).
Description of methods used in primary research
The studies included within the systematic review are quantitative empirical studies. The bulk of the
studies conducted used regression or structural equation modelling, as the poverty-related independent
variables tended to be on a continuous scale (e.g. distance from poverty line, income, education or
occupational status). A large proportion of the studies examined SES indicators as a covariate within more
complex models. Where this was the case, the inter-correlation matrices of variables were examined and
results were reported with no covariates. Many of the studies, particularly those looking at outcomes for
young children in poverty in the US, were secondary analyses conducted on large-scale national datasets.
Criteria for determination of independent findings
While the majority of studies covered single mechanisms of interest, there were a number of studies that
included both multiple measures of poverty-related variables and multiple measures of mechanisms. Our
coding frame included additional columns to capture designs in which up to two poverty variables had
reported relationships with up to two mechanisms. Where the studies were more complex, studies were
allocated new rows, though cross-referenced to the original study, so that the source of each observed
relationship was clear. Where there was one dataset that was analysed in multiple studies across papers
(such as the American Head Start dataset), resampling of the same group of participants was not judged
as a significant source of bias, as the sample size in such studies was very large (typically in the thousands).
Results and implications were developed based on a narrative review of the aggregate of studies, and
statistical meta-analyses were not conducted.
Details of study coding categories
Studies were coded by two researchers and each coded study was reviewed by the PI, in order to ensure
the greatest degree of accuracy. The categories that formed the coding frame are outlined in Table 6.
The results were captured and analysed in a spreadsheet, and overall trends were presented in narrative
form in the synthesis report. Where possible, potential mediators, moderators and covariates were
71
recorded to identify pathways, boundary conditions and confounds pertaining to the relationships
reported.
Table 6: Coding frame
72
Treatment of research outside of the scope
Grey literature was combined with a set of theoretical and review papers on the topic of poverty and
decision-making more broadly, in order to provide insight into the links between the mechanisms found
to be affected by poverty and decisions in key life domains, and also to inform recommendations for
intervention. Qualitative research papers were excluded from the primary review as the focus of this
review was already very broad, and case studies and monographs, while informative, are not robustly
generalisable. Papers using only qualitative methodology were stored in a separate database for
reference, with a view to offering richer insight and contextual details relevant to the research question
and the impacts of poverty in people’s lives.
73
Timeframe
Table 7: Illustrative timeframe of work undertaken
Plans for updating the review
Given the large scope of this review, it will not be practical for the authors to update this review on a
regular or ongoing basis. However, the authors intend to publish more detailed reviews concerning some
of the mechanisms in academic and open-access journals, and hope that this work forms the basis of
future academic enquiry.
Statement concerning conflict of interest
The authors do not observe any conflict of interest on their part, pertaining to the contribution of any
researcher to this project.
74
Acknowledgements
The authors are grateful to the Joseph Rowntree Foundation for supporting this research in its entirety.
They would also like to acknowledge the considerable efforts of Natasha Ann Brigham, Meherunissa
Hamid and Holly Minter, in conducting and assisting in the literature search, sourcing full-texts for review,
and coding. The team is grateful to Arnaud Vaganay for his methodological input throughout the project,
and for reviewing the Appendix. Enormous thanks are due to Professor Miles Hewstone and Ilona
Haslewood for their substantive guidance and oversight throughout the project, and thoughtful and
detailed feedback on a first draft of this report. The team is also appreciative of the review and comments
provided by Dave Innes at the Joseph Rowntree Foundation, and one anonymous external reviewer.
About the authors
Jennifer Sheehy-Skeffington is Assistant Professor of Social Psychology at the London School of
Economics and Political Science. She has published in social and political psychology on the topics of
intergroup inequality, ideological attitudes, conflict and poverty. Her research currently focuses on the
psychological consequences of low socioeconomic status in the context of rising income inequality, and
has received funding from the British Academy, the National Science Foundation and the Tobin Project.
Jessica Rea is a Research Associate at the London School of Economics and Political Science. Her PhD
studies (completed at Royal Holloway, University of London) centred on mental health and psychosocial
well-being among people dealing with homelessness and low socioeconomic status.
75
The Joseph Rowntree Foundation has supported this project as part of its programme of research and
innovative development projects, which it hopes will be of value to policy-makers, practitioners and
service users. The facts presented and views expressed in this report are, however, those of the author
and not necessarily those of JRF.
A pdf version of this publication is available from the JRF website (www.jrf.org.uk). Further copies of this
report, or any other JRF publication, can be obtained from the JRF website (www.jrf.org.uk/publications)
or by emailing [email protected]
A CIP catalogue record for this report is available from the British Library.
All rights reserved. Reproduction of this report by photocopying or electronic means for non-commercial
purposes is permitted. Otherwise, no part of this report may be reproduced, adapted, stored in a retrieval
system or transmitted by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, or otherwise without the
prior written permission of the Joseph Rowntree Foundation.
© LSE Enterprise Ltd 2017
First published February 2017 by the Joseph Rowntree Foundation
PDF ISBN 978 1 91078 382 5
Reference number: 3234
Joseph Rowntree Foundation
The Homestead
40 Water End
York YO30 6WP
www.jrf.org.uk
Inspiring Social Change
www.jrf.org.uk