Available online at www.scholarsresearchlibrary.com
Scholars Research Library
European Journal of Sports and Exercise Science, 2015, 4 (2):8-13
(http://scholarsresearchlibrary.com/archive.html)
ISSN: 2278 – 005X
8
Scholars Research Library
Visual skills of elite Brazilian football players
Marcello Alves
1
, Frank Spaniol
1
and Oscar Erichsen
2
1
Kinesiology Department, Texas A&M University Corpus Christi, USA
2
Clube Atlético Paranaense, Brazil
_____________________________________________________________________________________________
ABSTRACT
Several authors have reported the importance of visual skills (or sports vision) for sport performance, however,
little has been investigated about visual skills of football players. Thus, the purpose of the study was to investigate
the visual skills of elite Brazilian football players and compare visual skills scores of players from different age
groups and different positions. Fifty male elite Brazilian football players from a top football club from the main
Brazilian football league (first division) performed the visual skills test using Vizual EdgePerformance Trainer®
(VEPT) for the following visual skills: eye alignment, depth perception, convergence, divergence, visual recognition,
and visual tracking.No statistical significance was found between the age groups for eye alignment, depth
perception, convergence, divergence, visual recognition, and VEPT (combined total score) scores. Conversely,
statistical significance was found for visual tracking, in which the Pro (93.4 ± 2.2 %, P=0.05) and U20 (95.2 ± 1.8
%, P=0.00) were significantly better than the U15 (85.9 ± 1.8 %). In addition, the U20 (95.2 ± 1.8 %, P=0.03) was
also significantly better than the U17 (87.2 ± 2.1 %). With regards to players of different positions (goalkeepers,
defenders, midfielders and forwards), no statistical significance was found for any of the variables tested. In
conclusion, the present study reports that the visual skills, more specifically visual tracking, of more developed and
experienced elite football players (U20 and main squad) are greater than the visual skills of younger and less
experienced elite football players (U15 and U17).
Keywords: Visual skills, Sports Vision, Football, Soccer.
_____________________________________________________________________________________________
INTRODUCTION
Football, otherwise known as soccer, is without a doubt, the most popular sport in the world [1]. Along with the
enormous popularity of football, there has been remarkable expansion of football as a science. As times have
evolved, so has the general acceptance of contemporary scientific approaches being utilized during the preparation
of football players for competition. Such disciplines include physiology, biomechanics, strength and conditioning,
psychology, pedagogy, sociology, and nutrition [2]. In the last couple of decades there has been significant
accumulation of scientific data regarding football physiology and medicine [3]. However, little has been investigated
about visual skills of football players.
Several authors have reported the importance of visual skills (or sports vision) for sport performance [4-8]. Also,
many studies have shown significant differences in visual skills of athletes when compared to non-athletes [4, 9-11].
Regarding the type of sport, the majority of the studies in visual skills were done in baseball [12-15] and volleyball
[7, 10, 16-17]. Studies of visual skills in other sports such as tennis, table tennis, cricket, American football, and
rugby, can be fairly easily found as well. Nevertheless, very little can be found about visual skills in football,
especially for elite players.
Marcello Alves
et al Euro. J. Sports Exerc. Sci., 2015, 4 (2):
_____________________________________________________________________________________
9
Scholars Research Library
The few studies about visual skills done in football refer to goalkeepers [18-20] or referees [21-23]. Other studies,
with field players, were focused on kids rather than elite players [24].The research in these studies dealt with
perceptual skills, eye-foot coordination or reaction time instead of visual skills [25-30]. The rare studies found with
elite football players, investigated visual search (eye movement) instead of visual skills [31-32]. Thus, because of
the lack of information on visual skills of elite football players, more studies in the area should be done.
The purpose of the study was to investigate the visual skills of elite Brazilian football players. Further, the study also
aimed to compare visual skills scores of different age groups and different playing positions.
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Subjects
Fifty male elite Brazilian football players were tested. All subjects wereunder contract with a top football club from
the main Brazilian football league (first division). A minimum of tenplayers were randomly selected from each one
of the four age groups. Age groups were (1) U15 (14 and 15 years old), (2) U17 (16 and 17 years old), (3) U20 (18,
19 and 20 years old) and (4) Pro or first squad(players older than 20 years). Further, the subjects were also divided
into four different groups according to their field positions: (1) goalkeepers, (2) defenders (centre-backs and full-
backs), (3) midfielders, and (4) forwards.
Procedures
All fifty individuals performed the visual skills test using Vizual EdgePerformance Trainer® (VEPT).VEPT is a 3-D
computer-based visual skills training program that assesses the following visual skills: eye alignment, depth
perception, convergence, divergence, visual recognition, and visual tracking. The VEPT program provided
quantitative scores for each one of the visual skills mentioned previously, as well as a combined total VEPT score.
The tests consisted of four exercises using 3-D glasses (eye alignment, depth perception, convergence, and
divergence) and two exercises without the 3-D glasses (visual recognition and visual tracking). After detailed
instruction from the tester, each individual took the test only once. All tests were performed at the team’s training
center computer room.
Data analysis
Data analysis was done with SPSS statistics software® through multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA).
Further, the Tukey post-hoc test was performed to identify statistical differences. The level of significance
established was at P <0.05.
RESULTS
No statistical significance was found between the age groups for eye alignment, depth perception, convergence,
divergence, visual recognition, and VEPT scores (Table 1). Conversely, statistical significance was found for visual
tracking, in which the Pro (93.4 ± 2.2 %, P = 0.05) and U20 (95.2 ± 1.8 %, P = 0.00) were significantly better than
the U15 (85.9 ± 1.8 %). In addition, the U20 (95.2 ± 1.8 %, P = 0.03) was also significantly better than the U17
(87.2 ± 2.1 %). With regards to players of different positions (goalkeepers, defenders, midfielders and forwards), no
statistical significance was found for any of the variables tested (Table 2).
Table 1. Age groups and visual skill scores
Age Group N VT
(%)
SD
)
VR
(s)
SD
)
DP
(0-4)
SD
)
D
(%)
SD
)
C
(%)
SD
)
VEPT
(%)
SD
)
Pro 10 93.4* 4.4 1.52 0.7 1.7 0.9 86.1 11.4 84.9 17.9 68.1 8.6
U20 15 95.2** 5.2 1.56 0.4 1.4 0.9 89.8 5.5 92.3 6.7 69.8 4.8
U17 11 87.2 5.9 1.21 0.2 1.6 1.2 87.3 5.8 88.9 13.9 72.1 6.1
U15 14 85.9 10.1 1.65 0.5 1.1 0.8 82.7 22.8 83.5 20.1 65.6 8.6
Notes: VT – Visual Tracking, VR – Visual Recognition, DP – Depth Perception, D – Divergence, C – Convergence, VEPT – General
Visual Skill Score. Statistical significance found only for Visual Tracking. * The Pro group was significantly better than the U15 (P =
0.05). **U20 was significantly greater than the U17 (P = 0.03) and U15 (P = 0.00).
Marcello Alves
et al Euro. J. Sports Exerc. Sci., 2015, 4 (2):
_____________________________________________________________________________________
10
Scholars Research Library
Table 2. Positions and visual skill scores
Position N VT
(%)
SD
)
VR
(s)
SD
)
DP
(0-4)
SD
)
D
(%)
SD
)
C
(%)
SD
)
VEPT
(%)
SD
)
Forwards 14 88.6 6.9 1.74 0.6 1.4 1.1 79.6 23.6 83.5 20.8 66.3 9.7
Midfielders
17
93.1
1.37
89.2
90.1
11.5
69.9
Defenders
15
88.8
1.55
90.1
90.9
70.1
Goalkeepers 4 92.5 5.7 1.04 0.1 1.5 0.5 86.2 4.7 78.5 25.8 68.3 4.4
Notes: VT – Visual Tracking, VR – Visual Recognition, DP – Depth Perception, D – Divergence, C – Convergence, VEPT – General
Visual Skill Score. Statistical significance was not found.
DISCUSSION
Although some research studies show no benefits in sport performance from enhanced visual skills [33-34], the vast
majority of studies reported that developed visual skills (or sports vision) would play a positive role in sports
performance [4-8].Authors have reported that improvements in the athlete’s visual skills can lead to quicker
decision-making and faster motor response [35]. Thus, an advanced visual skills set is likely to have a positive effect
on athletes’ performance in many different sports. A study with rugby players showed an improvement in successful
catches by the use of a visual skills training protocol prior to a game [8]. In a study with NCAA Division I college
baseball players, significant differences in performance between athletes with visual skills training and without
visual skills training was discovered. The players with visual skills training produced significantly higher batted-ball
velocities than the players without visual skills training [12]. Likewise, in a study with table tennis players,
significant enhancement in performance was reported after eight weeks of visual skills training [36]. In addition, a
study with volleyball players indicated faster signal transmission in visual pathways in athletes with higher visual
skills [7]. Similar results were found with tennis players. Overney, et al. (2008), reported a positive relationship
between tennis performance and enhanced temporal processing [37]. Therefore several authors even suggested that
sports coaches should insert visual skills into their training programs to enhance athletic performance [6, 8, 36].
A large number of research studies in visual skills were focused on the comparison between athletes and non-
athletes. Most of these studies reported that athletes possess better visual skills than non-athletes [4, 9-11, 38-40]. In
a study comparing visual performances of athletes and non-athletes using a clinical battery of vision tests,
significantly better visual performances were found in the athletes for vergence facility, saccades, visual reaction
time, peripheral awareness, and near point of convergence [9]. Quevedo-Junyent, et al., (2011) reported that athletes
have a significantly higher dynamic visual acuity than non-athletes [40].
An enhanced visual skills set is not a privilege only for professional athletes. Student athletes have been found to
possess an enhanced visual skills set as well. A study with college students also found a significantly higher
dynamic visual acuity for the student athletes when compared to non-athlete students [39]. Another study with
college students reported that college baseball players possess a significantly higher visual search capacity than non-
athlete students [41].
Besides the comparison of athletes and non-athletes, there are also studies that suggest that athletes of different
levels differ on visual skills capacities as well [10, 42-43]. In a study with golfers it was found that high-level
players possessed significantly better visual skills than lower level players [43]. Jafarzadehpur et al., (2007)
compared visual skills of volleyball players of different levels [10]. The study found that as the playing level
increased, the visual skills capacities increased as well. In the present study, visual tracking was the only variable in
which statistical significance was found. Professional players (players from the main squad) had significantly greater
visual tracking than the players from the U15 squad. In addition, players from the U20 team had significantly better
visual tracking scores than the U15 and U17 players. These results suggest that older and more experienced players
possess greater visual tracking, which may be beneficial for their performance. These findings support similar
results found by Allen, et al. (2004) [44]. In their study, visual tracking of expert athletes was superior to novice
athletes. With regards to visual tracking, there are four major theories that try to explain the mechanisms of visual
tracking. Posner (1980) recognized that visual tracking is carried out by shifting a “spotlight” of attention
continuously from one target to the next [45]. Pylyshyn and Storm (1988) assumed that tracking is carried out pre-
attentively without the help of memory representations [46]. On the other hand, Yantis (1992) and Kahneman,
Treisman and Gibbs (1992)believed in high-level attentive processes, representations of objects, and spatial relations
between the tracked objects [47-48]. In most recent studies, several authors agree that visual tracking is highly
associated with visual memory, which relies critically on attention [47, 49-50]. Thus, more experienced players
Arman Jalili
et al Euro. J. Sports Exerc. Sci., 2015, 4 (2):8-13
_____________________________________________________________________________________
11
Scholars Research Library
would have an advantage by possessing a much larger motor memory when compared to younger athletes. With a
greater array of movement representations available, situation-specific reference structures for anticipatory control
would allow experienced athletes to have a better visual tracking performance when compared to less experienced
players [51].In addition to the long-term memory, short-term memory also plays a critical role in visual tracking.
According to Yantis (1992), subjects do not distinctly track the individual elements; instead, they can effectively
track numerousindividually moving elements. The target elements are maintained in short-term memory and are
rapidly updated as the elements move about the display [47].Furthermore, visual tracking performance is essentially
based on temporary spatial memory and attention switching [50].
The results of the present study also support the findings of two similar studies that compared visual search of
experienced versus inexperienced football players. Both of these studies found a significantly higher visual search
capacity of the experienced group [31-32]. Findings from Du Toit, et al. (2009), also corroborates with the results of
the present study. In their study, visual skills of football players were evaluated and showed that the higher the age
group/level, the better the visual skills were [24]. The same study also compared visual skills of players of different
positions, however no significant difference was found. Likewise, no statistical difference was found in the present
study for visual skills of players of different positions.
However, despite all the positive results found on visual skills of the research studies mentioned above, it is yet to be
known if enhancing visual skills of football players will result in increased football performance. A study with elite
Canadian football players concluded that improving players’ eye-foot coordination and other eye-linked motor
skills, failed to reflect an increase in football performance [30]. Moreover, several limitations of the present study
should be considered. The first limitation of the study wasthe difficulty to find related literature. Very few studies
were done in visual skills of football players and none of them used the instrument of the present study (Vizual
Edge). Thus, most of the literature reviewed is indirectly related, which makes the discussion of the results a
difficult task. Another limitation of the present study was the fact that players from only one team were tested. To
make better inferences about the population (elite football players) it would be necessary to have samplesfrom
different teams and countries. Future studies with different teams in different countries are strongly
suggested.Finally, the greatest limitation of the present study was related to the instrument. Vizual Edge is a
computer-based visual skills software and some subjects presented a more developed ability to use the keyboard
than others. Therefore, the results could have been influenced by how familiar a subject was with computers and
keyboards.
CONCLUSION
In conclusion, the present study reports that the visual skills of more developed and experienced elite football
players (U20 and main squad) are greater than the visual skills of younger and less experienced elite football players
(U15 and U17). More specifically, visual tracking was the visual skill in which statistical significance was found.
Therefore, experienced players may benefit from possessing more advanced visual tracking skills. However, more
research is necessary to understand if these differences would actually result in a better performance in the game
offootball. Also, these results suggest that visual tracking improves significantly in young elite football players
throughout their football career (from U15 to U20). Considering that these athletes never received specific visual
tracking training implies that elite football training in itself might be responsible for the development of visual
tracking skill. In addition, the present study also indicates that there are no differences in visual skills between
players of different positions. Finally, more research studies in this field are strongly recommended to better
understand the visual skills of elite football players and its implications in football performance.
REFERENCES
[1] Palacios-Huerta, I. Statistical Methods and Applications, 2004, 13(2), 241-258.
[2] Reilly, T., & Williams, M. Routledge, 2003, 2, 1-229.
[3] Ostojic, S. Research in Sports Medicine, 2004,12(2), 143-150.
[4] Hitzeman, S., & Beckerman, S. Optometry clinics: the official publication of the Prentice Society, 1993, 3(1),
145.
[5] Getz, D. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 1978,49(4), 385-388.
[6] Knudson, D., & Kluka, D. Journal of Physical Education, Recreation & Dance, 1997,68(4), 17-24.
Arman Jalili
et al Euro. J. Sports Exerc. Sci., 2015, 4 (2):8-13
_____________________________________________________________________________________
12
Scholars Research Library
[7] Zwierko, T., Osinski, W., Lubinski, W., Czepita, D., & Florkiewicz, B. Journal of Human Kinetics, 2010, 23(1),
21-17.
[8] Du Toit, P., Kruger, P., Fowler, K., Govender, C., Clark, J., & Clark, J. African Journal for Physical, Health
Education, Recreation and Dance, 2010, 16(3), 487-494.
[9] Christenson, G., & Winkelstein, A. Journal of the American Optometric Association.1988, 59(9), 666-675.
[10] Jafarzadehpur, E., Aazami, N., & Bolouri, B. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 2007,
17(2), 186-190.
[11] Zwierko, T. Journal of Human Kinetics, 2008, 19(1), 53–62.
[12] Spaniol, F., Bonnette, R., Ocker, L., Melrose, D., Paluseo, J., & Szymanski, D. Journal of Strength and
Conditioning Research.2008, 22(6), 112.
[13] Burroughs, W. International Journal of Sport Psychology.1984, 15(2), 117-126.
[14] Laby, D., Rosenbaum, A., Kirschen, D., Davidson, J., Rosenbaum, L., Strasser, C., &Mellman, M. American
journal of ophthalmology, 1996, 122(4), 476-485.
[15] Szymanski, J., Lowe, H., Szymanski, D., Cicciarella, C., Lowe, D., Gilliam, S., & Spaniol, F. Journal of
Strength and Conditioning Research, 2010,25(3): 49-50.
[16] Don Morris, G., & Kreighbaum, E. Research Quarterly. American Alliance for Health, Physical Education and
Recreation, 1977, 48(2), 480-483.
[17] Kokubu, M., Ando, S., Kida, N., & Oda, S. Perceptual and motor skills, 2006, 103(3), 709-716.
[18] Savelsbergh, G., Kamp, J., Williams, A., & Ward, P. Ergonomics, 2005, 48(11-14), 1686-1697.
[19] Hanvey, T. Advanced Cue Utilization of Soccer Goalkeepers.(Doctoral dissertation) University of British
Columbia.1999, Vancouver, Canada.
[20] Savelsbergh, G., Williams, A., Kamp, J.,& Ward, P. Journal of sports sciences, 2002, 20(3), 279-287.
[21] Ghasemi, A., Momeni, M., Rezaee, M., & Gholami, A. Journal of Human Kinetics, 2009, 22(1), 15–20.
[22] Loran, D. Optician,1999, 5702(217), 34-38.
[23] Ghasemi, A., Momeni, M., Jafarzadehpur, E., Rezaee, M., & Taheri, H. Perceptual and motor skills, 2011,
112(1), 161-171.
[24] Du Toit, P., Kruger, P., Chamane, N., Campher, J., & Crafford, D. African Journal for Physical, Health
Education, Recreation and Dance, 2009, 15(4), 594-604.
[25] Williams, A. Journal of Sports Sciences, 2000, 18(9), 737-750.
[26] Montes-Mico, R., Bueno, I., Candel, J., & Pons, A. Journal of the American Optometric Association, 2000,
71(12), 775-780.
[27] Vaeyens, R., Lenoir, M., Williams, A., & Philippaerts, R. Journal of Motor Behavior, 2007, 39(5), 395-408.
[28] Ward, P., & Williams, A. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 2003, 25(1), 93-111.
[29] McLeod, B. Perceptual and motor skills, 1991, 72(3), 863-866.
[30] Cohn, T., & Chaplik, D. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1991, 72(3c), 1238-1238.
[31] Williams, A., Davids, K., Burwitz, L., & Williams, J. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 1994, 65(2),
127-135.
[32] Williams, A., & Davids, K. Research quarterly for exercise and sport, 1998, 69(2), 111-128.
[33] Abernethy, B., & Neal, R. Journal of Science and Medicine in Sport, 1999, 2(1), 1-19.
[34] Abernethy, B., & Wood, J. Journal of sports sciences, 2001, 19(3), 203-222.
[35] Du Toit, P., Kruger, P., Mahomed, A., Kleynhans, M., Jay-Du Preez, T., Govender, C., &Mercier, J. African
Journal for Physical, Health Education, Recreation and Dance, 2011, 17(3), 429-440.
[36] Paul, M., Biswas, S., & Sandhu, J. Brazilian Journal of Biomotricity, 2011,5(2), 106-116.
[37] Overney, L. S., Blanke, O., & Herzog, M. H. Plos one, 2008, 3(6), e2380.
[38] Melcher, M., & Lund, D. Journal of the American optometric Association, 1992, 63(7), 466.
[39] Ishigaki, H., & Miyao, M. Perceptual and motor skills, 1993, 77(3), 835-839.
[40] Quevedo-Junyent, L., Aznar-Casanova, J.,Merindano-Encina, D., Cardona, G., & Solé Fortó, J. Research
quarterly for exercise and sport, 2011, 82(4), 644-651.
[41] Takeuchi, T., & Inomata, K..Perceptual and motor skills, 2009, 108(3), 971-980.
[42] Williams, A., & Grant, A. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 1999, 30(2), 194-220.
[43] Coffey, B., Reichow, A., Johnson, T., & Yamane, S. Visual performance differences among professional,
amateur, and senior amateur golfers.In Cochran, A., & Farrally, M. (Editors). Science and Golf II: Proceedings of
the World Scientific Congress of Golf,2003, 203. UK: Taylor & Francis.
[44] Allen, R., McGeorge, P., Pearson, D., & Milne, A. B. Applied cognitive psychology, 2004, 18(3), 337-347.
[45] Posner, M. I. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology.1980, 32, 3-25.
[46] Pylyshyn, Z. W., & Storm, R. W. Tracking multiple independent targets: Evidence for a parallel tracking
Arman Jalili
et al Euro. J. Sports Exerc. Sci., 2015, 4 (2):8-13
_____________________________________________________________________________________
13
Scholars Research Library
mechanism. Spatial Vision, 1988, 3, 1-19.
[47] Yantis, S. Cognitive Psychology, 1992, 24, 295-340.
[48] Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. J. Cognitive Psychology, 1992, 24, 174-219.
[49] Cavanagh, P., & Alvarez, G. A. Trends in cognitive sciences, 2005, 9(7), 349-354.
[50] Oksama, L. &Hyona, J. Visual. Cognition,2004, 11, 631–671.
[51] Schack, T., & Mechsner, F. Neuroscience letters, 2006, 391(3), 77-81.