Applied Psychology in Everyday Life
Applied Psychology in Everyday Life
Edited by
Bart L. Weathington,
Christopher J. L. Cunningham,
Brian J. O’Leary and Michael D. Biderman
Applied Psychology in Everyday Life,
Edited by Bart L. Weathington, Christopher J. L. Cunningham, Brian J. O’Leary
and Michael D. Biderman
This book first published 2011
Cambridge Scholars Publishing
12 Back Chapman Street, Newcastle upon Tyne, NE6 2XX, UK
British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data
A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library
Copyright © 2011 by Bart L. Weathington, Christopher J. L. Cunningham, Brian J. O’Leary
and Michael D. Biderman and contributors
All rights for this book reserved. No part of this book may be reproduced, stored in a retrieval system,
or transmitted, in any form or by any means, electronic, mechanical, photocopying, recording or
otherwise, without the prior permission of the copyright owner.
ISBN (10): 1-4438-3188-3, ISBN (13): 978-1-4438-3188-8
T
ABLE OF
C
ONTENTS
Preface....................................................................................................... vii
Psychology and Health: Aspects of the Mind that Affect Everyday
Health .......................................................................................................... 1
Michael B. Johnson
Psychology and Sports: From Theory to Practice...................................... 21
Mark H. Anshel
Psychology and Child Development: The Impact of Sport Participation
on Child Development............................................................................... 37
Kimberly S. Fasczewski, Emily S. Pica and David F. Ross
Psychology and the Law: The Good, The Bad, and The Ugly
of Eyewitness Testimony and Lineup Identification ................................. 57
Emily S. Pica, David F. Ross and Richard M. Metzger
Psychology and Religion: Explorations in Paradigm, Theory,
and Method................................................................................................ 71
Christopher F. Silver
Psychology and Work: Understanding People in the Workplace............ 109
Paul M. Muchinsky, Brian J. O’Leary and Bart L. Weathington
Psychology and Money: Pay, Motivation, and Financial Decisions........ 125
Bart L. Weathington and Jeanie G. Weathington
Contributors............................................................................................. 141
P
REFACE
When the word psychology is mentioned, most people automatically
think of mental illness. Words like "shrink" and “psychoanalysis” quickly
come to mind. When asked to name a psychologist, the most common
responses are Freud, Jung, Dr. Phil, or even Frasier. With a few notable
exceptions, the media perpetuates the view that all psychologists are
healthcare professionals concerned with mental health and well-being.
Movies, television, and books present psychologists as specialists who are
called in to deal with deviant or non-normal individuals. The field of
psychology, however, is associated with much more than just the
diagnosis and treatment of clinical psychological problems. While this
remains an important and essential function of psychology, many
psychologists have chosen to focus their time and research on issues and
problems associated with otherwise normal human functioning in all
domains of life.
A basic definition of psychology is the study of mind and behavior of
humans and animals in all environments. Accordingly, you will find
psychologists studying animals, children, adolescents, older adults, groups
and teams, jobs and work, and virtually every other aspect of human life.
Applied psychology practitioners and research psychologists work in
academia, industry, and government roles. You will find psychologists
performing activities as diverse as teaching, consulting to industry, and
testifying before Congress. Psychologists and psychological researchers
have impacted virtually all of us in some manner. From employment
testing to athlete training, to the design of human-machine interfaces,
psychological research underlies many areas of life that impact "normal"
people.
The American Psychological Association (www.apa.org) currently has
divisions recognizing 54 subfields of psychology. While these divisions
are by no means unique and many psychologists belong to multiple
divisions, the number of recognized subfields provides evidence of the
diversity of psychological activity. Psychology is a diverse field that has
much to offer society in terms of both theoretical and practical knowledge.
The study of psychology is not only for those interested in understanding
what makes some people different from others. It is also for those who
wish to understand what makes all of us the same.
Preface viii
This book showcases a variety of applications of non-clinical
psychological knowledge. It is an outgrowth of the River Cities Industrial-
Organizational Psychology Conference held at The University of
Tennessee at Chattanooga in October 2008. The theme from the
2008conference was “Applying Psychology to Everyday Life” and we
believe the following chapters effectively illustrate the wide scope and
impact of applied psychology. We hope you enjoy this material and are
intrigued by the enormous potential for psychology to impact our everyday
lives.
BART L. WEATHINGTON
CHRISTOPHER J. L. CUNNINGHAM
BRIAN J. O’LEARY
MICHAEL D. BIDERMAN
June 2011
Chattanooga, TN
P
SYCHOLOGY AND
H
EALTH
:
A
SPECTS OF THE
M
IND THAT
A
FFECT
E
VERYDAY
H
EALTH
M
ICHAEL
B
J
OHNSON
,
T
HE
U
NIVERSITY OF
T
ENNESSEE AT
C
HATTANOOGA
Everybody brings his or her own unique perspectives and views to life.
Each person’s perceptions are based on that individual’s interpretations of
two highly interactive factors: the environment (i.e., external to the
person) and that person’s subjective interpretations of the environment.
And it is a person’s perceptions that can impact his or her physical,
mental, and emotional health (e.g., seeing a snake in the backyard can be
anxiety provoking for one person, yet not of concern to another). A
person’s idiosyncratic subjective interpretations of the environment
include unique evaluations of the environment itself, and of that person’s
evaluation of his or her ability to cope with that environment. Moreover,
coping includes beliefs regarding what needs to be done in any given
situation (e.g., dealing with the external environment) and beliefs
addressing the ability one has to handle the emotions associated with an
event (i.e., dealing with one’s internal environment).
The primary goal of this chapter is to provide some insight into the
very complex perceptual system (Taylor, 2003) and its impact of physical
health through a discussion centering on an integrative, simple, systemic,
and inclusive model that involves four psychological constructs or factors.
The model will be discussed factor by factor, and then systemically, all the
while identifying links with health. This is followed by a brief overview of
individual strategies for enhancing psychological and physical health.
Individuals are impacted by their personal experiences and how
prepared they sense they are to effectively manage such experiences. For
example, drug abuse can be the behavior selected by an individual who
wishes to avoid a subjectively perceived hopeless life situation, or enhance
subjectively perceived experiences. In either case, drug abuse is highly
likely to increase the chances of negative physical and/or psychological
outcomes. In many cases, physical and mental health can be enhanced via
Psychology and Health 2
positive interactions with others (e.g., support, empathy) and a better
understanding of one’s internal processes (e.g., the use of coping strategies
and increased self-awareness) in ways that respect the individual’s
lifestyle and culture. I begin this chapter by addressing some of the
challenges people may encounter when attempting to interact positively
with the environment.
Challenges to Healthy Living
Psychological factors influence physical health in many different ways.
For example, many Western cultures rely on a short-term perspective or
focus almost exclusively on objective outcomes. These, as well other
general and specific life stressors (e.g., job loss), can degrade physical
health (Hobfoll, Schwarzer, & Chon, 1998). That is, research supports a
link between stress and many health outcomes such as anxiety disorders,
bipolar disorder, depression, heart disease, the common cold, and impaired
immune response (Gray, 2011). The human body automatically responds
to subjectively perceived threats via its sympathetic nervous system. This
is the portion of the nervous system that readies the body to face a threat
or run away (i.e., fight or flight).
The physiology of the “fight or flight” response includes an increased
heart rate and the release of sugars and fats into the blood stream to
enhance physical activity. This response can be quite beneficial to health,
but only in the short term. At times we all experience situations that
require immediate attention (e.g., your child falls and needs to be taken to
the doctor for stitches) and in such instances activating the “fight or flight”
system is helpful toward achieving a given goal. However, when the
sympathetic nervous system is constantly activated (e.g., stressful job or
poor interpersonal relationship) adverse physical and psychological
consequences can occur, including exhaustion and depression (Selye,
1936, 1976). Therefore, investigating the causes of constant stress in
people’s lives is worthwhile. For example, sources of stress often can be
considered a function of one’s perceptions of an event more than they can
be considered the result of the actual objective event. Better understanding
how this happens can lead to more effective preventative and curative
interventions.
Whenever individuals operate with perceptions, goals, values, and
interests in a systemic manner that are incongruent with positive mental
and emotional health, stress can result. How an individual subjectively
interprets his or her life events, as well as his or her internal mental and
emotional experiences (i.e., how one feels about something, someone, or
Michael B. Johnson 3
some event), are common areas of interest in psychology. Often how one
perceives himself or herself is a source of that person’s strengths and
weaknesses. In order to provide a well structured argument for the linkage
between one’s psychological and physical well-being, the current chapter
presents a model that describes one way of psychologically approaching
the relationship between (a) physical health and (b) thoughts and feelings.
A Model Linking Psychology and Physical Health
The model around which this chapter’s contents are presented was
derived specifically from a model of cultivating mental balance proposed
by Wallace and Shapiro (2006). The model includes four psychological
factors (conation, attention, cognition, and affect) and their systemic
interactions (Figure 1). Imbalances (i.e., too much or too little) in any of
these four, or a dysfunctional relationship among them, can result in stress,
which is directly related to physical health (e.g., Selye, 1936, 1976).
Psychological
Health
Conation
Attention
Cognition
Affect
Physical
Health
The Individual
The Environment
Figure 1. A model that conceptually links psychological health and physical health
via the constructs of conation, attention, cognition, and affect; as well as these
constructs’ systemic interactions with the environment.
Psychology and Health 4
The model presented in Figure 1 illustrates the dynamics that occur
within a person. Additionally, this dynamic interplay likely is involved in
the quality of that individual’s psychological experiences. Furthermore,
these subjective experiences can play a role in an individual’s
psychological and physical health. As such, the benefits of achieving a
balance within each and among the four are elaborated upon next.
Conation
Conation involves volition (e.g., desires and preferences) and intention
(i.e., goal directed behavior). Both volition and intention are essential
aspects of happiness and satisfaction (Rabten, 1992; Rinpoche, 2003).
When one experiences a conative deficit he or she is apathetic, unmotivated,
or feels highly complacent. This experience can result in depression and a
lack of engagement in professional, social, and interpersonal aspects of
life. At the other end of the spectrum from a conative deficit is conative
hyperactivity, which can be thought of as a type of obsessiveness that
often leads to a great deal of unhappiness. Obsession or perfectionism can
lead to long hours of work (e.g., workaholic) or overtraining in sports, for
example, which quite possibly leads to burnout in either domain.
Additionally, selfish and highly egocentric behaviors can result from
conative hyperactivity, which likely harm one’s well-being and the well-
being of others who are integral to that person’s life (e.g., significant other,
colleagues, friends, family). Therefore, when either form of conative
dysfunction exists, an individual’s level of volition and intention impair
his or her well-being, and/or the well-being of others.
Intrapersonal (i.e., within the individual) conative balance, by definition,
occurs when an individual moves toward ambitions that are reality-based
and lead to his/her and othershappiness (Emmons, 1986). An individual
moves toward conative balance by engaging in reflection, improving his or
her awareness of the relationship between personal goals and the
environment, and developing an understanding of the appropriateness of
his or her current coping strategies relative to specific life challenges. This
awareness raising improves psychological well-being and is the basis for a
number of counseling psychology interventions. Essentially, when a
person can improve his or her understanding of the “why” underlying
behaviors (i.e., self-awareness) while accurately identifying those behaviors
that are under volitional control, then that person likely will experience
improved quality of life.
Individuals from all walks of life can benefit from improving their
ability to identify the personally meaningful reasons for their chosen
Michael B. Johnson 5
behaviors. Once sufficient self-awareness has been reached an individual
can move toward behaviors that lead to or are part of a truly positive life
experience.
An improved understanding of why one engages in certain behaviors
(e.g., self-awareness) has great significance in many domains relevant to
the human experience, including work, sports, religion, and social
relationships. Motivation, which is strongly related to conation, is often
identified as a primary and necessary feature of accomplishment in many
domains (Gould, Dieffenbach, & Moffett, 2002). Additionally, the type of
motivation one emphasizes is important. Research has identified that
focusing solely on outcomes weakens one’s motivation or desire to
participate and engage the environment (Deci, Betley, Kahle, Abrams, &
Porac, 1981; Vallerand, Gauvin, & Halliwell, 1986). However, even when
individuals experience an objective loss (e.g., being fired from work),
having positive interpersonal support (e.g., colleagues and teammates) can
enhance motivation (Tauer & Harackiewicz, 2004). It appears that
focusing on the experience of the process (e.g., interpersonal interactions)
has benefits. Emphasizing a process-orientation while simultaneous
acknowledging the existence of an outcome-orientation, can move an
individual toward positive life experiences, even if a particular individual
has an elevated ego or self-centered focus (Brunel, 2000; Pensgaard &
Roberts, 2002, Roberts, 2001).
It appears that individuals’ subjective desires and unique purposes
underlying their behavior play a role in whether or not their life
experiences are perceived positively or negatively. Recognizing one’s
ambitions in life, identifying any of these that are dysfunctional, and
considering the possibility that changes are needed due to existing
dysfunctional conations, likely will lead an individual toward a happier
and healthier life. Once an individual identifies clear, specific, measurable,
adjustable, realistic, and time-bound goals that link well with his or her
values and interests, that person likely will enhance his or her ability to
attend to salient aspects of life. This leads to a discussion of the next
psychological factor in the model – attention.
Attention
To engage meaningfully in any human experience, attention must be
paid to that experience (Ryan & Deci, 2001; Teasdale et al., 2000). During
an attentional deficit, an individual is unable to focus on the task at hand
or the desired object. Alternatively, when one’s mind is racing too quickly,
or when one is overly engaged in multiple tasks simultaneously, then
Psychology and Health 6
attentional hyperactivity results, which impedes that person’s positive life
experience. Excessive arousal, agitation, or distraction are highly probable
during attentional hyperactivity and can lead to an unenjoyable life
experience (Critchley & Mathias, 2003). Essentially, attentional hyperactivity
involves, by definition, excessive concentration on aspects of life that
likely result in harm, and those aspects of life can include inappropriate
objects, individuals, behaviors, or thought processes (Cohen & Blum,
2002).
The ability to self-regulate one’s thought processes, behavior, and
decision making appropriately is defined as attentional control. Support for
the positive impact of being able to appropriately regulate one’s levels of
attention (i.e., attentional balance) in life is extensive (see Abernethy,
Maxwell, Masters, van der Kamp, & Jackson, 2007 for a review). The
psychological state of flow, which occurs when one experiences a high
level of attention and commitment to an activity at that moment
(Csikszentmihalyi, 1990), is an example of attentional balance. When in a
flow state a person feels energized, positive, and as though he or she is one
with the task. Additionally, time feels as though it is altered in a way that
allows the individual to merge his or her actions with self-awareness. This
flow state is congruent with attentional balance. It occurs when the
individual immerses himself or herself in an activity solely for the sake of
the activity, and the results can include positive life experiences, happiness,
and elevated levels of performance. Being able to simultaneously relax
and intently attend to a particular task or goal, as occurs during a flow
state, appears to enhance people’s health (e.g., lowering arousal). An
experience consisting of high levels of attention and low levels of arousal
may seem paradoxical, yet research investigating the benefits of these
seemingly congruent intrapersonal experiences has not been definitive and
appears worthy of future study. Nevertheless, it has been shown that one’s
ability to attend to what is occurring in the environment (i.e., attentional
focus), whether internal or external, has a strong relationship with that
person’s perceived happiness and objective performance level (Simons &
Chabis, 1999).
Being aware of to what one is attending (i.e., thinking) is an aspect of
the human experience that is psychological in nature and yet is directly
linked to physical health (e.g., lowered levels of stress; Wang et al., 2005).
Therefore, one’s thoughts (i.e., cognitions) also likely play a salient role in
that individual’s psychological and physical well-being. Building on this
discussion, cognition is discussed next.
Michael B. Johnson 7
Cognition
Cognitions can essentially be considered thoughts. An individual who
experiences cognitive balance is calmly and clearly in the present,
experiencing life moment by moment. That is, during cognitive balance
there is a heightened sense of knowing that comes from within the
individual, rather than the existence of pure thought. An individual in a
state of cognitive balance is able to accept events without misinterpreting
them (e.g., cognitive distortions). Alternatively, a cognitive deficit is
considered highly similar to absent-mindedness, and cognitive hyperactivity
is akin to being unable to discriminate among various real and fantasy
laden experiences. Therefore, one experiences cognitive dysfunction when
he or she is unable to identify some “thing” as congruent with that thing’s
objective reality, resulting in an outcome that is harmful in some way to
either the perceiver or the object of the perception (Rabten, 1992). At its
most basic level, cognitive dysfunction is similar to one misinterpreting a
life experience (e.g., a family conversation, the boss’s off-hand comment)
due to faulty or inaccurate thought processes. One reason that individuals
periodically incorrectly perceive others’ (and possibly their own)
emotions, attitudes, or intentions is that they fail to recognize their own
biases, hopes, fears, and concerns (i.e., projections).
Cognitive imbalances are something with which everyone is familiar.
Thought processes that enhance one’s observational and perceptual skills
can lead to an improved quality of life. Improving one’s understanding of
the personal meaningfulness of life’s experiences (i.e., self-awareness) is
one way to do this. Shifting toward greater self-awareness can be
challenging. There are often barriers to enhanced self-awareness, and these
barriers are usually self-imposed. Many people can easily say to
themselves, “I am really mad at him because of what he said,” however,
few people are aware of the illogical cause and effect relationship within
such a statement. The intrapersonal response of “mad” is not caused by the
statement, but by the listener’s interpretation. That is, addressing
dysfunctional cognitions may not involve changing the content of one’s
thoughts, but changing one’s relationship with those thoughts. The
perceived stress resulting from, “…what he said” comes from within the
listener, not the speaker. The listener has chosen to internally develop “I
am… mad.” The speaker could not make the listener feel this way. This
leads to a discussion of the last component of the model at the heart of this
chapter. This fourth factor, affect, involves how one’s experiences are
recognized, perceived, and processed at a feeling level.
Psychology and Health 8
Affect
Affect can be defined as the aspect of the human experience that is
subjectively identified and processed at a feeling level. Affect can be
thought of as an umbrella term under which all human emotions fall. That
is, affective experiences include subjective levels of arousal and
pleasantness, and discrete emotions can each be plotted within a matrix
defined by the two continua of arousal and pleasantness (Figure 2).
Affective balance implies a lack of excessive emotional fluctuations
(affective hyperactivity) and a lack of emotional apathy (affective deficit;
Wallace, 2005). Essentially, affective dysfunction is marked by emotional
expression that is inappropriate for the situation (i.e., either too much or
for too long) resulting in a negative impact on one’s life experiences
(Goleman, 1997, 2003). For example, research has identified that one’s
affective states influence his or her performance in ways unique to each
individual (Johnson, Edmonds, Tenenbaum, & Kamata, 2007) and training
individuals to self-regulate their affective intensity can impact performance
(Edmonds, Tenenbaum, Mann, Johnson, & Kamata, 2008). Moreover,
these latter two studies illuminate the systemic nature of the human
experience, which is addressed later in this chapter.
Arousal
Pleasantness
Sad
Happy
Contentment
Angry
low
high
low high
Figure 2. A model illustrating the two continua that comprise affect, with four
examples of discrete emotions inserted within the model.
Michael B. Johnson 9
Individuals experiencing affective dysfunction can benefit from a
deeper relationship with their empathic selves (Shapiro, Schwartz, &
Bonner, 1998). A heightened level of gratefulness is one characteristic of
an empathic individual (Emmons & McCullough, 2003). Additionally,
research supports the argument that an individual can positively impact his
or her well-being when he or she is able to experience delight in one’s own
intrinsic worth and successes, as well as others’, regardless of the
individual’s self-interests (e.g., enjoying a colleague’s promotion party at
work when you were up for the same position) (Salzberg, 2002; Wallace,
2004). Developing concepts such as empathy and equanimity can improve
an individual’s psychological and physical well-being. This chapter now
moves to a discussion of the systemic aspect of the model.
An Integration of Conation, Attention, Cognition, and Affect
Human beings’ experiences and perceptions via their conative, attentive,
cognitive, and affective selves have been briefly outlined. It is important to
have an understanding of the systemic relationship among these factors
prior to being able to develop a holistic view of the human experience as it
relates to psychological and physical health.
As an example of the systemic complexities involved in the human
experience I share an excerpt from an actual interaction that occurred
within a counseling session. A client of mine once expressed a great deal
of frustration due to remembering important issues and topics three to four
days after his weekly counseling sessions. When I pointed out that this
also was a case of thinking of issues and topics three or four days
preceding his next weekly session he expressed surprise. He had not
considered this way of thinking. His conative intensity was functional
while his attention and cognitions were not, leading to affective states that
disrupted his ability to maintain a positive life experience. He was
frustrated and unhappy. Subsequent work with this client focused on
continuing to improve his self-awareness, and then on strategies he could
implement that would move him toward more balanced attentional and
cognitive styles. Sometimes a person’s quality of life (e.g., his or her
happiness) can be impaired by his or her dysfunctional thought processes
and attentional foci.
Happiness, in a truly holistic sense, is not simply a universal state of
pleasure derived from external sources (e.g., money, social accolades), or
getting what you think you want. One’s level of happiness is related to the
emotional quality of one’s perceptions. Studies have identified that money,
for example, can buy happiness, but only up to a point. Kahneman and
Psychology and Health 10
Deaton (2010) provide an analysis of data from 450,000 Americans in
2008 and 2009 and concluded that beyond an income of $75,000 per year,
individuals are not happier. In fact, it is possible that the added stress of
keeping “all your stuff” safe may negatively impact happiness and
physical health. Additionally, those who are poor experience higher levels
of stress and sadness, with lower levels of happiness. However, these low
levels of happiness rise with increasing income, but this relationship
experiences diminishing returns, especially when an individual surpasses
an annual income of approximately $75,000.
This income plateau as it relates to levels of intrapersonal happiness is
possibly due to the differences in perspective between those who make
over $100,000 per year and those who bring in, say, $50,000 a year. High
income earners may be less able to appreciate the “small things” in life
(Quoidbach, Dunn, Petrides, & Mikolajczak, 2010) than those who
struggle to make ends meet. It is important to point out a few facts
regarding these studies as well, and these additional points highlight the
complex system involved in the relationship between psychological and
physical health. First, there is a difference between happiness and life
evaluation (i.e., the thoughts people have about their lives) (Kahneman &
Deaton, 2010). Life evaluation primarily involves one’s thoughts about the
overall quality of one’s life, which differs from one’s perceived general
happiness. Life evaluations include personal evaluations of the overall
goodness of one’s life (e.g., the quality of one’s house, children’s schools,
car, clothes, or social position in the community). Evaluations of
happiness, on the other hand, are linked specifically with physical health,
and include people’s responses to how frequently they feel joy, sadness,
anger, and other emotional experiences.
Other studies addressing the relationship between happiness and
income level have shown similar results. North and colleagues (2008)
identified that family income has a weak correlation with happiness, but
that family social support has a much stronger impact. Interestingly, North
et al. discovered that at lower levels of income, family social support has a
stronger relationship with happiness than at high levels of family income.
That is, family social support is not as strong a predictor of happiness in
rich families as it is in poor families. Additionally, Diener, Ng, Harter, and
Arora (2010) exposed income as a fairly robust predictor of life satisfaction
(i.e., satisfaction with one’s standard of living), but a relatively weak
predictor of subjective happiness. And it is these latter psychological
factors that are related strongly with stress, and stress levels are directly
associated with physical health (Selye, 1936, 1976).
Michael B. Johnson 11
Studies addressing the relationship between people’s levels of
subjective happiness and their income (Diener et al., 2010; Kahneman &
Deaton, 2010; North et al., 2008; Quoidbach et al., 2010), as well as
anecdotal reports from counseling sessions, help illustrate the impact that
one’s conations, attention, cognitions, and affect can have on that person’s
quality of life. If any of the four factors is dysfunctional (e.g., desiring a
job that earns $1 million annually), then elevated stress levels may result
(e.g., decreased happiness) due to the time away from family or
extraordinary demands on one’s personal time. Such stress could very well
result in poor physical health (Selye, 1936, 1976). Psychological skills
training designed to assist a person with identifying dysfunctional conations,
attention, cognitions, and affect (i.e., self-awareness training) is a logical
first step toward decreasing one’s stress, thereby improving physical
health. It is an overview of these mental regimens that I present next.
Putting These Concepts into Everyday Life
Having briefly presented research supporting a strong connection
between psychological health and physical health (Selye, 1936, 1976),
then presented a model that includes four psychological factors related to
psychological health, I now provide a few real world psychological skills
based on this chapter’s model. These skills can be applied to everyday life,
and may thereby enhance one’s physical health.
A number of concepts can help link the factors presented thus far and
the real world. Self-efficacy (Bandura, 1986, 1989), defined as one’s
beliefs about his or her ability to effectively manage a current challenge, is
the first link discussed. At its most basic level, the concept of self-efficacy
can be summarized by the adage, “Whether you think you can or think you
can’t, you’re right.” An individual’s self-efficacy regarding his or her
intrapersonal processes (i.e., thoughts, feelings, or physiological responses)
can have a noticeable impact on that person’s subjectively perceived
quality of life (Eklund, 1994, 1996). In addition to self-efficacy, an
individual’s ability to establish and recognize a clear self-identity also
correlates with enhanced psychological well-being (Marcia, 1994). As
individuals improve their self-awareness, their self-identity becomes
clearer. Furthermore, both an individual’s self-identity and self-awareness
are influenced by his or her developmental stage. For example, adolescents
experience a different set of challenges when seeking to improve self-
efficacy, self-identity, or self-awareness than do middle age adults, as
these two groups differ in their cognitive development and emotional
development. And lastly, one’s gender, race, ethnicity, and sexual
Psychology and Health 12
orientation also will factor into how one interacts with the environment.
So, essentially, there is no one “right” way to approach positive
psychological health, and thus no single approach to positive physical
health. The following skills can be generalized to most people, and these
involve self-efficacy, self-identity, and self-awareness, all of which are
based on conation, attention, cognition, or affect.
Psychological Skills
Skills designed to advance an individual’s ability to embrace his or her
intrapersonal experiences (i.e., facilitating an improved understanding of
his or her thoughts, feelings, and actions) can have a large impact on that
individual’s subjectively experienced quality of life. A variety of skills are
discussed briefly here, including goal setting, adaptive perfectionism,
coping with and controlling anxiety levels, attention enhancing, and level
of interpersonal competence. Implementing the aforementioned
psychological skills in a manner that help one improve his or her
psychological quality of life is highly recommended as improved physical
health is likely to result. While discussing each of these skills I specifically
identify how each skill relates with conative, attentional, cognitive, and
affective factors.
Goal setting. An important skill to mention first is goal setting, which
includes any activity that helps focus attention; increases effort, intensity,
and persistence; and facilitates problem solving (Locke & Latham, 1990).
Lines of evidence supporting a strong causal relationship between goal
setting and quality of life exist in a number of domains (e.g., Brobst &
Ward, 2002; Burton, 1989; Kingston & Hardy, 1997). Moreover, goal
setting does not have to include hours of planning or reams of
documentation. What is critical is that the identified goals be concrete,
flexible, controllable, challenging, and measurable (Burton & Naylor,
2002).
Goals can be either outcome- or process-oriented, and there are very
real therapeutic benefits to maintaining a balance between the two. This is
because a solely or predominately outcome goal orientation may not be
fully controllable (e.g., the goal to “get a raise at work”) and as such may
have a negative impact on that very outcome and the person’s psychological
health. However, outcome goals are often a source of motivation.
Moreover, outcome goals are adjustable and therefore can be altered as
circumstances dictate (e.g., a downturn in the economy may dictate
changing the goal of getting a raise at work).
Michael B. Johnson 13
Although outcome goals can be beneficial at times, process goals are
vital in most every situation. Process goals frequently result in improved
self-awareness and increased attention to factors such as effort and the
“how” of life experiences (e.g., the goal of discovering “how I can do my
job better”). Focusing on process goals allows one to engage in the event
itself, the benefits of which are addressed by the concept of flow (Jackson
& Csikszentmihalyi, 1999). To retain focus “in the moment,” (i.e., a
process orientation) one needs to be comfortable with self-evaluation. For
example, analyzing one’s level of outcome and process foci may lead to
reframing if outcomes are receiving a dysfunctional amount of the
individual’s attention.
Effectively achieving one’s goals, whether they are process, outcome,
intrapersonal, interpersonal, or group as a whole, is a matter of an
individual’s perceptions. It is these perceptions that can be problematic or
helpful and, therefore, should be the focus of goal setting. If an
individual’s goals appear to be problematic, the cause may be related to
expectations, which can be held by the individual or others (e.g., family,
friends, the press). Identifying the nature of one’s distress and then
working through that experience is a process fundamental to successful
goal setting.
Adaptive perfectionism. The understanding of one’s self and one’s
relationship with environmental experiences plays a vital role in quality of
life. Possessing an ability to accept that some factors are under one’s
control, while others are not (i.e., adaptive perfectionism), is part of
adaptive perfectionism. Listening to one’s self-talk can shed light on one’s
level of adaptive perfectionism. Emphasizing accurate and positive self-
talk is a cognitive skill that can be beneficial. That is, when a person is
able to identify an inner dialogue, and then accurately and objectively
clarify what he or she feels is controllable and that which is not, could
prove invaluable for individuals who wish to improve their quality of life.
Being able to improve upon one’s ability to focus, plan, problem solve,
and think about situations and the self in positive and accurate terms is
important.
Affective coping strategies. Experiencing dysfunctional levels of
affect is a challenge that can be addressed effectively in a number of ways.
For example, affective hyperactivity (e.g., being excessively emotional)
likely would entail learning a relaxation strategy, of which there are many.
An individual who is able to identify accurately his or her challenge as
being overly emotional (e.g. excessive ruminations leading to elevated
anxiety) will benefit from a mental relaxation technique more than he or
she will from a technique that addresses somatic symptoms. Cognitive
Psychology and Health 14
coping strategies include (a) developing a routine, (b) implementing a
cognitive restructuring plan (i.e., changing how one thinks about a
situation), or (c) employing positive thought control (i.e., when a negative
or self-defeating thought occurs, stop it and replace it with a positive
thought). On the other hand if one identifies that his or her stress is
primarily bodily (i.e., somatic), then physical relaxation strategies (e.g.,
progressive muscle relaxation, breathing control exercises) are indicated
(Maynard & Cotton, 1993; Maynard, Hemmings, & Warwick-Evans,
1995; Maynard, MacDonald, & Warwick-Evans, 1997).
Overall, there is support in the literature for the effectiveness of active
problem-focused coping strategies (e.g., cognitive reappraisal, positive
self-talk, the use of breathing exercises, and implementing a pre-event
routine) in both the short and long term, rather than avoidance techniques
(e.g., disengaging). This latter approach is not encouraged. Disengaging
may have short term benefits but will have long term disadvantages
relative to achieving satisfaction, experiencing enjoyment, and continued
engagement in life’s activities (Kim & Duda, 2003).
Attention-enhancing skills. Appropriately selecting and sustaining
attention has been shown to be vital to engagement in and quality of life
(Kitsantas & Zimmerman, 2002; Orlick & Partington, 1988). Some
aspects of life require high levels of attention, while others require none at
all (i.e., automatic tasks). That is, less attention should be paid to the more
basic motor skills. This allows greater focus toward other, more
situationally important factors. Appropriately selecting those aspects of
life on which one focuses is paramount to an individual’s quality of life.
Interpersonal competence. In addition to intrapersonal factors (i.e.,
those within the individual), focusing on enhancing interpersonal
competence can positively impact quality of life (e.g., a social support
system; Holt & Dunn, 2004). Among the various interpersonal
relationships one can experience, perhaps the most significant in people’s
lives is the family and/or one’s relationship with a significant other. Some
of the most stressful life experiences involve events such as marriage,
divorce, or the death of a family member (Holmes & Rahe, 1967). This is
so because, in most cases, life involves at least two people’s mutually and
causally interdependent thoughts, feelings, and behaviors (i.e., a dyadic
relationship), and frequently each of these can involve adjustment on the
part of those involved in the relationship. Change is difficult and often
stressful. However, the quality of an interpersonal relationship tends to
depend upon the emotional tone of the relationship (e.g., fondness, trust,
and respect); the congruence of the relevant individuals’ goals, values, and
hopes; and the level of cooperation amongst those in the relationship.
Michael B. Johnson 15
Interpersonal hurdles that often present themselves include distrust,
conflicting goals, and power struggles. Actively and accurately addressing
these hurdles increases the chance of a quality interpersonal experience.
Psychological Skills Conclusion. The skills briefly reviewed herein
are not exhaustive. The number of possible skills is large. For the most
part, we are limited solely by our creativity. Any skill that leads a person
to increased levels of optimism, confidence, and hope is recommended.
Conclusion
This chapter began by stressing the importance of perceptions and their
relationship with one’s quality of life. Although objective reality matters,
it is the quality of one’s perceptions of that objective reality that is unique
to that person. And these perceptions are identifiable via our conations,
attention, cognition, and affect. Moreover, these four factors are at least
minimally controllable. By actively addressing conations, attentional
focus, cognitions, and level of affective intensity, people play a role in
improving their psychological health, thereby reducing negative life
experiences (e.g., stress levels) and subsequently positively impacting
physical health and overall quality of life.
References
Abernethy, B., Maxwell, J. P., Masters, R. W., van der Kamp, J., &
Jackson, R. C. (2007). Attentional processes in skill learning and
expert performance. In G. Tenenbaum & R. C. Eklund (Eds.),
Handbook of sport psychology (3
rd
ed., pp. 245-263). Hoboken, NJ:
Wiley.
Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought and action. A social
cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
—. (1989). Social cognitive theory. In R. Vista (Ed.), Annals of child
development (Vol. 6, pp. 1-60). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.
Brobst, B., & Ward, P. (2002). Effects of public posting, goal setting, and
oral feedback on the skills of female soccer players. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 35, 247-257.
Brunel, P. (2000). Achievement motivation: Toward interactive effects of
dispositional and situational variables on motivation and social
cognition. Habilitation a diriger les researches. France: University of
Limoges Press.
Psychology and Health 16
Burton, D. (1989). Winning isn’t everything: Examining the impact of
performance goals on collegiate swimmers’ cognitions and performance.
The Sport Psychologist, 3, 105-132.
Burton, D., & Naylor, S. (2002). The Jekyll/Hyde nature of goals:
Revisiting and updating goal-setting in sport. In T. S. Horn (Ed.),
Advances in sport psychology (2
nd
ed., pp. 459-499). Champaign, IL:
Human Kinetics.
Cohen, J. D., & Blum, K. I. (2002). Overview: Reward and decision.
Introduction to special issue. Neuron, 36, 193-198.
Critchley, H. D., & Mathias, C. J. (2003). Blood pressure, attention and
cognition: Drivers and air traffic controllers. Clinical Autonomic
Research, 13, 399-411.
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Flow: The psychology of optimal experience.
New York, NY: Harper & Row.
Deci, E. L., Betley, G., Kahle, J., Abrams, L., & Porac, J. (1981). When
trying to win: Competition and intrinsic motivation. Personality and
Social Psychology Bulletin, 7, 79-83.
Diener, E., Ng, W., Harter, J., & Arora, R. (2010). Wealth and happiness
across the world: Material prosperity predicts life evaluation, whereas
psychosocial prosperity predicts positive feeling. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 52-61. doi: 10.1037/a0018066
Edmonds, W. A., Tenenbaum, G., Mann, D. T. Y., Johnson, M., &
Kamata, A. (2008). The effects of biofeedback training on affective
regulation and simulated race car performance: A multiple case study
analysis. Journal of Sport Sciences, 26, 761-773.
Eklund, R. C. (1994). A season-long investigation of competitive
cognition in collegiate wrestlers. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
Sport, 65, 169-183.
—. (1996). Preparing to compete: A season-long investigation with
collegiate wrestlers. Sport Psychologist, 10, 111-131.
Emmons, R. A., & McCullough, M. E. (2003). Counting blessings versus
burdens: Experimental studies of gratitude and subjective well-being in
daily life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 377-389.
Goleman, D. (1997). Healing emotions: Conversations with the Dalai
Lama on mindfulness, emotions, and health. Boston, MA: Shambhala.
—. (2003). Destructive emotions: How can we overcome them? A
scientific dialogue with the Dalai Lama. New York, NY: Bantam Dell.
Gould, D., Dieffenbach, K., & Moffett, A. (2002). Psychological
characteristics and their development in Olympic champions. Journal
of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 172-204.
Gray, P. (2011). Psychology (6
th
ed.). New York, NY: Worth Publishers.
Michael B. Johnson 17
Hobfoll, S. E., Schwarzer, R., & Chon, K. K. (1998). Disentangling the
stress labyrinth: Interpreting the meaning of the term stress as it is
studied in a health context. Anxiety, Stress, and Coping, 11, 181-212.
Holmes, T. H., & Rahe, R. H. (1967). The social readjustment rating scale.
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 11, 213-218.
Holt, N. L., & Dunn, J. G. (2004). Toward a grounded theory of the
psychosocial competencies and environmental conditions associated
with soccer success. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16, 199-
219.
Jackson, S., & Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1999). Flow in sport. Champaign,
IL: Human Kinetics.
Johnson, M. B., Edmonds, W. A., Moraes, L. C., Filho, E. S., &
Tenenbaum, G. (2007). Linking affect and performance of an
international level archer: Incorporating an idiosyncratic probabilistic
method. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 317-335.
Johnson, M. B., Tenenbaum, G., Edmonds, W. A., & Castillo, Y. (2008).
A comparison of the developmental experiences of elite and sub-elite
swimmers: Similar developmental histories can lead to differences in
performance level. Sport, Education and Society, 13, 453-475.
Kahneman, D., & Deaton, A. (2010). High income improves evaluation of
life but not emotional well-being. Proceedings of the National
Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 16489-
16493. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011492107
Kim, M. S., & Duda, J. (2003). The coping process: Cognitive appraisals
of stress, coping strategies, and coping effectiveness. The Sport
Psychologist, 17, 406-425.
Kingston, K. M., & Hardy, L. (1997). Effects of different types of goals on
processes that support performance. The Sport Psychologist, 11, 277-
293.
Kitsantas, A., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2002). Comparing self-regulatory
processes among novice, non-expert, and expert volleyball players: A
microanalytic study. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 14, 91-105.
Locke, E. A., & Latham, G. P. (1990). A theory of goal setting and task
performance. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
Marcia, J. E. (1994). Ego identity and object relations. In J. M. Masling &
R. F. Bornstein (Eds.), Empirical perspectives on object relations
theory (pp. 59-93). Washington, DC: American Psychological
Association.
Maynard, I. W., & Cotton, P. C. J. (1993). An investigation of two stress-
management techniques in a field setting. Sport Psychologist, 7, 375-
387.
Psychology and Health 18
Maynard, I. W., Hemmings, B., & Warwick-Evans, L. (1995). The effects
of a somatic intervention strategy on competitive state anxiety and
performance in semiprofessional soccer players. Sport Psychologist, 9,
51-64.
Maynard, I. W., MacDonald, A. L., & Warwick-Evans, L. (1997). Anxiety
in novice rock climbers: A further test of the matching hypothesis in a
field setting. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 28, 67-78.
North, R. J., Holahan, C. J., Moos, R. H., & Cronkite, R. C. (2008).
Family Support, Family Income, and Happiness: A 10-Year
Perspective. Journal of Family Psychology, 22, 475-483. doi:
10.1037/0893-3200.22.3.475
Orlick, T., & Partington, J. (1988). Mental links to excellence. Sport
Psychologist, 13, 458-469.
Pensgaard, A. –M., & Roberts, G. C. (2002). Elite athletes’ experiences of
the motivational climate: The coach matters. Scandinavian Journal of
Medicine and Science in Sport, 12, 54-60.
Quoidbach, J., Dunn, E. W., Petrides, K. V., & Mikolajczak, M. (2010).
Money giveth, money taketh away: The dual effect of wealth on
happiness. Psychological Science, 21, 759–763.
Rabten, G. (1992). The mind and its functions (S. Batchelor, Trans.; 2
nd
ed.). Mont Pèlerin, Switzerland: Editions Rabten Choeling.
Rinpoche, C. T. (2003). Parting from the four attachments. Ithaca, NY:
Snow Lion.
Roberts, G. C. (2001). Understanding the dynamics of motivation in
physical activity: The influence of achievement goals on motivational
processes. In G. C. Roberts (Ed.). Advances in motivation in exercise
and sport (pp. 1-50). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (2001) On happiness and human potentials: A
review of research on hedonic and eudaimonic well-being. Annual
Review of Psychology, 52, 141-166.
Salzberg, S. (2002). Lovingkindness: The revolutionary art of happiness.
Boston, MA: Shambhala.
Selye, H. (1936). A syndrome produced by diverse nocuous agents.
Nature, 138, 32.
—. (1976). The stress of life. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Shapiro, S. L., Schwartz, G. E., & Bonner, G. (1998). Effects of
mindfulness-based stress reduction on medical and premedical
students. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 21, 581-599.
Simons, D., & Chabris, C. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained
inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 1059-1074.
Michael B. Johnson 19
Tauer, J. M., & Harackiewicz, J. M. (2004). The effects of cooperation and
competition on intrinsic motivation and performance. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 849-861.
Taylor, S. E. (2003). Health Psychology (5
th
ed.). New York, NY:
McGraw-Hill.
Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z. V., Williams, J. M., Ridgeway, V. A., Soulsby, J.
M., & Lau, M. A. (2000). Prevention of relapse/recurrence in major
depression by mindfulness-based cognitive therapy. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 68, 615-623.
Vallerand, R. J., Gauvin, L., & Halliwell, W. R. (1986). Negative effects
of competition on children’s intrinsic motivation. Journal of Social
Psychology, 126, 649-657.
Wallace, B. A. (2004). The four immeasurables: Cultivating a boundless
heart (Rev. ed.). Ithaca, NY: Snow Lion.
—. (2005). Genuine happiness: Meditation as the path to fulfillment.
Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
Wallace, B. A., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). Mental balance and well-being:
Building bridges between Buddhism and Western psychology.
American Psychologist, 61, 690-701.
Wang, J., Rao, H., Wetmore, G. S., Furlan, P. M., Korczykowski, M.,
Dinges, D. F., & Detre, J. A. (2005). Perfusion functional MRI reveals
cerebral blood flow pattern under psychological stress. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,
102, 17804-17809. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0503082102
P
SYCHOLOGY AND
S
PORTS
:
F
ROM
T
HEORY TO
P
RACTICE
M
ARK
H.
A
NSHEL
M
IDDLE
T
ENNESSEE
S
TATE
U
NIVERSITY
How often do you observe athletes fail to perform at a desirable level
under pressure, not deal with frustration very well, admit to feeling
nervous before or during a contest, fail to cope with stress very well, or
beat an opponent who was heavily favored? What about coaches whose
leadership style consists primarily of yelling or threatening his or her
athletes, or having the athletes perform exercises as punishment for
something they did or did not do? These are just a few examples of
situations in sport that are influenced by an athlete’s thoughts or emotions,
that is, “the mental game.”
Successful athletes differ psychologically from their less successful
counterparts in their mastery and proper application of an array of mental
skills that allow them to anticipate success, concentrate on the task at
hand, transfer skills and strategies from practice into competition, and
overcome adversity, which is inherent in competitive sport. Sport
psychology deals with the psychological factors that influence physical
performance including sport, exercise, work, rehabilitation, and the arts
(e.g., dance, acting, performing music).
The field of sport psychology means different things to different
people, depending on one’s background and interests. Sport psychology
can be an area of clinical practice, for instance, treating athletes for an
anxiety disorder, as an educator (e.g., teaching a course or seminar), as a
team consultant (e.g., mental skills coach), as a researcher (e.g., a
university professor who enjoys the study of psychology in a sport
context), or the use of sport psychology concepts and techniques in
applied settings by athletes, coaches, and athletic trainers. Sport
psychology has evolved into a reputable area of science and practice, not
unlike the larger field of general psychology. Sport psychologists are
focused on understanding the factors that help explain reaching and
consistently maintaining optimal performance, not only in sport, but in
Psychology and Sports 22
other physical performance settings. Let’s review some components of the
field.
In a field (applied) setting, sport psychology is often referred to as “the
mental game,” and practitioners are often called mental skills coaches. The
applied nature of sport psychology concerns the use of mental skills
examples of such skills include psyching up, attentional focusing, cueing,
or visualization - or understanding the psychological factors that influence
sport performance. Practitioners apply this information to improve the
competitor’s thoughts and emotions, leading to better performance
outcomes. Top athletes bring to every competition a full arsenal of mental
skills and strategies they know will make a difference between winning
and losing, between success and failure, and sometimes those outcomes
are a matter of a single incident. Look at the pre-performance rituals of
golfers, baseball batters and pitchers, and basketball free throw shooters
before executing the skill.
Sport psychology principles and concepts originated with the field of
psychology. Psychology concepts such as arousal, intrinsic/extrinsic
motivation, team member interaction (also called group dynamics),
leadership skills and styles (coaching psychology), communication skills,
aggression, causal attributions (i.e., the motivational value of explaining
the causes of outcomes), and clinical issues (e.g., sleeplessness, anxiety,
drug or alcohol addiction, burnout, low compliance with injury
rehabilitation) are just a few examples of areas commonly studied in
psychology that have been studied and applied by sport psychology
researchers and practitioners.
Finally, sport psychology educators, consultants, and researchers often
attempt to explain, describe or predict behavior. An example of explaining
behavior includes observing an athlete’s inability to perform up to
expectations or in accordance with previous performance level under high
pressure conditions. This is referred to as “choking” and there are specific
strategies that athletes can use to prevent or overcome the choke.
Describing behavior may involve focusing on the pre-performance
routines athletes often display prior to executing a skill, let’s say warm up
or mental practice, or some other series of thoughts or actions that prepare
the athlete for the task at hand (e.g., baseball hitters who first unwrap, then
rewrap their batting gloves between pitches). Predicting behavior is
defined as the degree of accuracy with which a researcher, practitioner, or
coach can accurately anticipate a particular performance outcome given
the presence and, sometimes, interaction, of one or more factors. For
example, it can be accurately predicted that if a coach teaches a skill or
strategy in an angry manner, the athlete will not retain the coach’s